We use cookies and similar technologies to improve your website experience and help us understand how you use our website. By continuing to use this website, you consent to the usage of cookies. Learn more about our Privacy Statement and Cookie Policy .

  • Our Mission
  • Code of Conduct
  • The Consultants
  • Hours and Locations
  • Apply to Become a Consultant
  • Make an Appointment
  • Face-to-Face Appointments
  • Zoom Appointments
  • Written Feedback Appointments
  • Support for Writers with Disabilities
  • Policies and Restrictions
  • Upcoming Workshops
  • Class Workshops
  • Meet the Consultants
  • Writing Guides and Tools
  • Schedule an appointment! Login or Register
  • Graduate Students
  • ESOL Students
  • The Three Common Tenses Used in Academic Writing

  He explains the author’s intention and purpose in the article.

*He is explaining the author’s intention and purpose in the article.

Both of the sentences above are grammatically correct. However, the tense used in first sentence (present simple) is more common for academic writing than the tense in the second sentence (present progressive). This handout provides the overview of three tenses that are usually found in academic writing. 

There are three tenses that make up 98% of the tensed verbs used in academic writing. The most common tense is present simple, followed by past simple and present perfect. These tenses can be used both in passive and active voice. Below are the main functions that these three tenses have in academic writing.

The Present Simple Tense

Present simple is the most common tense in academic writing, and it is usually considered as the “default” unless there is a certain reason to choose another tense (e.g. a sentence contains a past time marker). Some specific functions of present simple include:

The Past Simple Tense

Generally, past simple is used to refer to actions completed in the past. Some specific functions this tense has in academic writing include:

The Present Perfect Tense

Present perfect is usually used when referring to previous research, and since it is a present tense, it indicates that the findings are relevant today. More specifically, this tense might have the following functions:

Common Questions about Tense in Academic Writing

Question: Can tenses change in the same paragraph or sentence?

Explanation: Yes, there are some times where it is appropriate to switch tense within a paragraph or sentence. However, you have to have a good reason for it.  For instance, a shift in time marked by an adverb or prepositional phrase (e.g. since, in 2013, until ) or when you move from general statements to specific examples from research (one of the functions mentioned above).

Question : Are other verb tenses used in academic writing?

Explanation : Yes, although not as common, other tenses are used in academic writing as well. For example, when expressing strong predictions about the future, the future simple tense is used, or when describing events that undergo changes at the time of writing, present progressive is used.

Read the excerpt and notice the tenses used for each verb. Identify the function of each tense as illustrated in the first sentence.  

Approximately 10% of the population is diagnosed (present simple, function 4) with dyslexia (Habib, 2000). Specialized testing most often reveals this disability in third grade or later, when there develops an observable differential between reading achievement and IQ (Wenar & Kerig, 2000). This late identification poses severe problems for effective remediation. At the time of diagnosis, poor readers are on a trajectory of failure that becomes increasingly difficult to reverse. Attempts at intervention must both focus on remediation of the impaired components of reading as well as extensive rehabilitation to reverse the growing experience differential. 

Educators and researchers are aware of the need for early diagnosis. In response, research investigating early correlates of later reading ability/disability has burgeoned (e.g. Wagner et al., 1997). However, these early reading studies primarily focus on school age children (e.g. Share et al., 1984). To date, only a few studies have focused on the reading trajectories of children younger than preschool, and there is little consistency within the existing studies (e.g. Scarborough, 1990, 1991). 

In the current study, we trace the development of the two aspects of the phonological processing deficit in a longitudinal follow-up study of two-year-olds. Shatz et al. (1996, 1999, 2001) investigated the underlying lexical structure in two-year-old children. Although their experiments were tailored to examine early word learning behavior, their study design is uniquely suited to looking at the phonological processing skills of two-year old children as well. In this study, we measure the early reading skills of these same two-year-olds at five to seven years of age in order to determine the predictivity of the early two-year old behaviors for later reading ability. 

Adapted from Michigan Corpus of Upper-level Student Papers. (2009). Ann Arbor, MI: The Regents of the University of Michigan.

Approximately 10% of the population is diagnosed (pres. simp. F4) with dyslexia (Habib, 2000). Specialized testing most often reveals (pres. simp. F4) this disability in third grade or later, when there develops (pres. simp. F4) an observable differential between reading achievement and IQ (Wenar & Kerig, 2000). This late identification poses (pres. simp. F3) severe problems for effective remediation. At the time of diagnosis, poor readers are (pres. simp. F3) on a trajectory of failure that becomes (pres. simp. F3) increasingly difficult to reverse. Attempts at intervention must both focus on remediation of the impaired components of reading as well as extensive rehabilitation to reverse the growing experience differential. 

Educators and researchers are (pres. simp. F1) aware of the need for early diagnosis. In response, research investigating early correlates of later reading ability/disability has burgeoned (pres. perf. F1) (e.g. Wagner et al., 1997). However, these early reading studies primarily focus (pres. simp. F3) on school age children (e.g. Share et al., 1984). To date, only a few studies have focused (pres. perf. F3) on the reading trajectories of children younger than preschool, and there is (pres. simp. F3) little consistency within the existing studies (e.g. Scarborough, 1990, 1991). 

In the current study, we trace (pres. simp. F2) the development of the two aspects of the phonological processing deficit in a longitudinal follow-up study of two-year-olds. Shatz et al. (1996, 1999, 2001) investigated (past. simp. F1) the underlying lexical structure in two-year-old children. Although their experiments were tailored (past. simp. F1) to examine early word learning behavior, their study design is uniquely suited (pres. simp. F3) to looking at the phonological processing skills of two-year old children as well. In this study, we measure (pres. simp. F2) the early reading skills of these same two-year-olds at five to seven years of age in order to determine the predictivity of the early two-year old behaviors for later reading ability. 

The information in this handout is adapted from Caplan, N. (2015). Grammar choices for graduate and professional writers . Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press. 

Last updated 12/20/2017

Grammar & Style

  • Converting Fragments to Full Sentences
  • Active and Passive Voice
  • Choosing Between Infinitive and Gerund: “To do” or “doing”?
  • Choosing the Correct Word Form
  • Combining Clauses to Avoid Comma Splices, Run-ons, and Fragments
  • Commas, Semicolons, and Colons
  • Count vs. Noncount Nouns
  • Definite and Indefinite Articles
  • Improving Cohesion: The "Known/New Contract"
  • Modal Verbs
  • Parallel Structure
  • Prepositions
  • Proper Nouns
  • Reducing Informality in Academic Writing
  • Run-on Sentences
  • Same Form, but Different Functions: Various Meanings of Verb+ing and Verb+ed
  • Subject-Verb Agreement
  • Using Reduced Relative Clauses to Write Concisely
  • Verb Tenses
  • Word Order in Statements with Embedded Questions

George Mason University Logo

The Writing Center

4400 University Drive, 2G8 Fairfax, VA 22030

Quick Links

  • Register with us

© Copyright 2024 George Mason University . All Rights Reserved. Privacy Statement | Accessibility

IMAGES

  1. Verb Tenses: How to Use the 12 English Tenses Correctly • 7ESL

    explanation text use tense

  2. What is an Explanation Text?

    explanation text use tense

  3. 12 Types of Tenses With Examples Pdf

    explanation text use tense

  4. Verb Tenses: How to Use the 12 English Tenses Correctly • 7ESL

    explanation text use tense

  5. 25 Examples of Simple Present Tense Sentences

    explanation text use tense

  6. Tenses Examples (58 Sentences of all tenses)

    explanation text use tense

VIDEO

  1. Learn Tenses In English Grammar With Examples

  2. NARRATIVE TEXT#GENERICSTRUCTURE#TENSE#V2

  3. TUGAS PRESENTASI PRESENT CONTINUOUS TENSE, Narrative Text

  4. Understanding "Tense Atmosphere": A Guide for English Learners

  5. What's Tense || Tense vs Zero Tense || English for CSS and PMS

  6. Eng02-Examples of sentences using the present simple tense

COMMENTS

  1. The Writing Center

    This handout provides the overview of three tenses that are usually found in academic writing. Background. There are three tenses that make up 98% of the tensed verbs used in academic writing. The most common tense is present simple, followed by past simple and present perfect. These tenses can be used both in passive and active voice.