Studying With Music: Arguments for & Against

Does music really help you study?

  • By Sander Tamm
  • Aug 4, 2021

E-student.org is supported by our community of learners. When you visit links on our site, we may earn an affiliate commission.

At some point, almost every student has experimented with using background music as a means to study or  work  more efficiently. 

It is no wonder, then, that the iconic YouTube channel  Lofi Girl  is currently approaching the 1B views mark.

But, does music actually make a legitimate difference in the effectiveness of an individual study session?

According to relevant science, listening to music while studying does have its advantages as well as significant disadvantages – one thing is for sure, there is no concrete “yes or no” answer to whether music affects studying positively.

If we leave the science out of it and look at the individual opinions of students, things stay just as complicated – many students swear by background music, while many learners are completely against ANY background noise.

The debate on whether music can help you study or not tends to boil down to two concrete arguments.

One side has the opinion that listening to music helps  improve focus and concentration . The other camp says that they don’t want  any additional noise  because their own thoughts provide enough distraction as it is. Thus, for these students, music seems to make concentration even more difficult.

In this article, we will be looking at both the science and the individual experience of the students. In addition, we will explore both the advantages and disadvantages of listening to music while studying.

Hopefully, the article will let you decide on which option would suit you better –  studying in complete silence or doing the brain crunching with some light background music on.

Without further ado, let’s get started by looking at some advantages of listening to music while studying.

Here’s what you’ll find in this article:

Studying with Music

Arguments for listening to music while studying.

Here are four major advantages of listening to music while studying:

Relaxing music can combat study-related stress

Learning about a new subject or going through a demanding online course can be overwhelming and stressful, even for the brightest of minds. Therefore, it is essential (at least if you want the study session to be fruitful) to study with a positive mindset.

Why? Well,  research  suggests that positive affect (term used in psychology for positive emotions and expression) improves a variety of cognitive processes.

Simply put, you will learn better when you are in a good mood.

This is where listening to music comes in – according to scientists from  Stanford University , music can  change brain functioning  to the same extent as meditation.

It is a quick, easy, and cheap way of flooding your brain with endorphins and reaping the benefits of improved cognitive function immediately.

Music can help with completing repetitive tasks

According to a  study by Fox & Embrey  (1972), music can be a great productivity aid when you need to perform repetitive and more simple tasks.

For example, try listening to music when rewriting or editing a paper. Music can inspire you to tackle these somewhat tedious activities with greater efficiency.

Music can inspire you to tackle these somewhat tedious activities with greater efficiency. For example, try listening to music when rewriting or editing a paper. You’ll quickly see that you work more quickly and efficiently when listening to music.

Music can also make a boring activity seem less boring. Listening to music while doing something mundane, like creating  charting  tables or creating columns for  Cornell notes .

Music can help with memorization

In one way or another, all learning is about memorization. And, according to science, background music can have a  positive impact on human memory .

So, next time you are trying to memorize phrases from an  online language course , have some background music on. It just might give your learning that extra boost.

Listening to music with headphones can cancel noise pollution

In a perfect world, you could always choose exactly where and when you study. Unfortunately, that is not always the case.

There will be times when your study environment will work against you – distractions can come in the form of other family members, roommates, or traffic noise.

In such cases, putting on a pair of headphones would be a great way of fighting  noise pollution . After all, a soothing collection of tunes will be a much better background to your studies than hearing your roommate play video games for hours on end.

Arguments against listening to music while studying

These are the two main drawbacks to listening to music while studying:

Lack of concentration

Whether you consciously notice it or not, your brain will put some extra resources into “decoding” the music playing in the background. This is especially true for songs with lyrics – instead of focusing on your studies, you will unconsciously try to listen to what the singer is saying. This can ultimately  decrease your productivity  by 10%. Thus, for tasks that require cognitively demanding and creative mental work, complete silence would be the best possible solution.

Complete silence  promotes increased blood flow  to the brain. This, in return, directly affects your capability to tackle more demanding mental tasks.

So, if you are planning on mastering a hard skill like  PHP  or  Python , always try to find a place as silent as possible.

Music can trigger bad memories

Music can put you in a positive mood (thus enhancing your cognitive capabilities), but the reverse is also possible.

Some music can come with bad associations and lead you to a more negative state of mind. Which, in return, will dampen your learning capabilities.

You can combat this by avoiding music that is too dark or downbeat. Thinking about your high school heartbreaks is the last thing you want when studying for that Advanced Calculus exam.

4 Tips for using background music to enhance your mental capacity

If you are planning on experimenting with listening to music while studying, there are some things to consider. These tips will help you avoid some mistakes students commonly make when studying with background music.

Avoid music with lyrics!

It is not a coincidence that most of the music associated with studying is entirely instrumental.

According to a study by Perham/Currie (2014), music with lyrics has a negative effect on reading comprehension performance. The same study also finds that the same applies even when the student enjoys the music or already knows the lyrical content.

To conclude, do not choose albums, playlists, or songs with any lyrical content.

If you are not knowledgeable about instrumental music, the Lofi Girl YouTube channel mentioned in the introduction is the best place to start. For further inspiration, we will also list some music styles that we recommend experimenting with.

But, before we get into specific styles, allow me to explain why not all instrumental music works as a backdrop for a learning session.

Use background music that follows clear patterns

We already established that the debate is still on whether listening to music while studying is beneficial or not. But, one thing is clear – the  type of music  you choose for your study sessions matters!

Not any instrumental music is suitable background music by default. Music that is too progressive in nature is likely to throw your brain off the loop and distract you.

For example, listening to jazz while studying might “make sense” to some students, but jazz is quite chaotic in nature. Thus, I wouldn’t choose jazz as background music for studying, even if the pieces are entirely instrumental.

The lo-fi hip-hop beats have become such popular choices for study music because they generally follow a clear, defined rhythm. These beats provide a pleasant background ambiance that does not demand your brain for attention. 

Here are some other examples of music styles suitable for learning:

  • Ambient  – Brian Eno, possibly the most famous ambient composer of all time, has described ambient music as music that “induces calm and a space to think”. This is exactly what you want when selecting suitable music for getting some studying done.
  • Downtempo  – Similar to ambient, but with a bigger focus on beats. The atmospheric sounds and the mellow beats make downtempo a great backdrop for long study sessions.
  • Classical  –  The Mozart Effect  has been effectively  debunked . Listening to classical music does NOT make you smarter. Still, classical music can enhance your mood and put you in the right mind frame for studying. Reading while listening to classical is something that works particularly well for many students.
  • Deep house  – An unconventional choice, but something that has worked for me and several other students we interviewed. Deep house has very little variation, the beats are hypnotic, and the rhythms are soothing.

Of course, this is not an exhaustive list. What works for one student, might not work for someone else. So, feel free to experiment with different albums, playlists, and artists to see what works best for your study routine.

Make your choices before the learning sessions

You have probably heard that some of the most high-performing individuals in the world consciously limit their daily choices. This is a great strategy for avoiding decision fatigue as much as possible.

Decision fatigue is the last thing you want when engaging in mentally demanding work such as studying. Thus, if you are planning on studying with background music playing, choose your music well in advance. Preferably the day before your learning session.

Take regular breaks

You can use the stopping of music as a cue for a study break. For example, prepare separate playlists for your learning sessions. When one playlist is finished, take a break.

Since your brain has learned to associate music with studying, you will find it that much easier to relax and switch off when the music stops.

When you resume learning, your mind will be fully refreshed and ready to absorb new information.

The usefulness of background music for studying depends on many variables. It all comes down to the specific music listened to, what you are studying, and the environment where you study.

Of course, the personality and the study habits of a given student are also important.

Everyone has different preferences, so you will need to experiment with what works for you. Some people may find that listening to music while studying helps them focus more and retain information better. Others will do better in silence without any noise at all.

The scientific evidence is also inconclusive. Some studies show that music can help improve attention and memory, while other studies find no benefits for listening to music while studying.

If you do decide to experiment with having background music on while studying, there are some key takeaways to consider.

For one, anything with lyrics should be avoided. The same goes for music that is too loud and intense or too progressive. Instead, opt for music that is repetitive and classically pleasant.

Also, be sure to prepare your playlists or streams BEFORE starting studying. Choosing songs while engaging with your studies is a surefire formula for throwing your brain off the loop and hurting your concentration levels.

Sander Tamm

Fluentu spanish: does the youtube video method work.

FluentU Spanish is a good option for exploring entertainment subjects in Spanish. Although it can be a fun learning experience, it lacks structure and the coverage of different linguistic skills.

Handwritten vs. Typed Notes: Which Is Really Better?

Taking notes by handwriting vs. typing: which is really better?

10 Best Skillshare Classes for Designers & Creatives

Skillshare has a massive catalog of high-quality online courses. Here, you’ll find the best of the bunch.​

Texas A and M University College of Liberal Arts Logo

Does Listening to Music Really Help You Study?

Experts from the department of psychology explain whether or not music is a helpful study habit to use for midterms, finals, and other exams.

graohic of listening to music

By Mia Mercer ‘23

Picture of girl studying with headphones

Students have adopted several studying techniques to prepare for exams. Listening to music is one of them. However, listening to music may be more distracting than helpful for effective studying.

There’s no season quite like an exam season on a university campus. Students turn to varying vices to help improve their chance of getting a good grade. While some chug caffeine, others turn up the music as they hit the books.

Although listening to music can make studying more enjoyable, psychologists from the Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences have found that this popular study habit is more distracting than beneficial. 

“ Multitasking is a fallacy; human beings are not capable of truly multitasking because attention is a limited resource, and you can only focus on so much without a cost,” cognitive psychologist Brian Anderson said. “So when you’re doing two things at the same time, like studying and listening to music, and one of the things requires cognitive effort, there will be a cost to how much information you can retain doing both activities.” 

In basic terms of memory, Anderson explained that we do a better job of recalling information in the same conditions in which we learn the material. So when studying for an exam, it’s best to mimic the exam conditions. 

“If you have music going on in the background when you study, it’s going to be easier to recall that information if you also have music on in the background when you take the exam,” Anderson said. “However wearing headphones will almost certainly be a violation during most exams, so listening to music when you’re studying will make it harder to replicate that context when you’re taking an exam.” 

Even though experts suggest listening to music can hinder your ability to retain information while studying, some students choose to continue the practice. Steven Smith, cognitive neuroscientist for the Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences , provided some suggestions for students who wish to continue this study habit. 

 “In general, words are distracting,” Smith shared. “So if you want to listen to music while you study, try to listen to something that does not have words, or if it does have words, hopefully, it’ll be in a language that you don’t understand at all, otherwise that’s going to distract from the stuff you’re trying to study.”

Smith also suggested listening to familiar background music, because it’s less distracting than something new or exciting. Additionally, Smith provided some principles that generally result in better exam results. 

“Make sure your studying is meaningful because comprehension gets you so much further than raw repetition,” Smith shared. “Also, you must test yourself, because it’s the only way you can learn the material; this is called the testing-effect. And finally, try to apply the spacing-effect, where you spread out your study sessions rather than cramming your studying all together, allowing for better memory of the material.”

Regardless of how students decide to study for exams, it’s important to remember that we all learn differently.

“There are individual differences between everyone,” Smith said. “Some people need a study place that is boring, predictable, and exactly the same so that they can concentrate, and others find it more beneficial to go to different places to study. It’s true that there are different personalities, so try and find what study habit works best for you.” 

  • The College at Work
  • Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences
  • Preparing for Exams

The Effects of Listening to Music While Studying Research Paper

Introduction.

Music is a human construct, because humans have to acknowledge and define the existence of an objective reality of sound into music. Every human culture uses music to promote its ideas and ideals and music is intrinsically interwoven in the fabric of each society. Music is considered as a powerful tool for shaping individual abilities and character, and “musicality is a universal trait of humankind.” (Hallam, Susan, 2006). “If sounds are created or combined by a human being, recognized as music by some group of people and serve some functions which music has come to serve for mankind, then those sounds are music.”(Radocy and Boyle, 1988, p.19).

With the advent of modern electronic gadgets students are exposed to more and more avenues of entertainment and study time is not devoid from the interference of these gadgets. How music, particularly background music, affect student learning is an area attracting much research, and it is finding difficult to produce conclusive evidence to support this habit.

The Oxford Dictionary defines music as “the art of combining sounds of voice(s) or instrument(s) to achieve beauty of forms and expression of emotion.” The effect of music on individual to individual will be at variance as it depends on subjective judgments of what constitutes beauty of form and expression of emotion of an individual. Miller (2000) argues that:

“music exemplifies many of the classic criteria for a complex human evolutionary adaptation. He points out that no culture has ever been without music (universality); musical development in children is orderly; musicality is widespread (all adults can appreciate music and remember tunes); there is specialist memory for music; specialized cortical mechanisms are involved; there are parallels in the signals of other species—for example, birds, gibbons and whales—so evolution may be convergent; and music can evoke strong emotions, which implies receptive as well as productive adaptations” (Hallam, 2006, p.2).

Many historical evidences show that music existed many thousands of years ago and several musical instruments were developed in different parts of the world. According to theorization of Huron (2003), music developed among different cultures as a part of courtship behavior, social cohesion, group effort, perceptual development, motor skill development, conflict reduction, safe time passing, and a mnemonic device of trans-generational communication.

Music has multiple functions which influence development of individuals, social groups and the society as a whole. Music as a medium of expressing human feelings transcend into enforcing social norms and continuity and stability of culture, at the same time contributes to the integration of society. In the perspective of an individual music is a medium for emotional expression particularly when words and verbal exchanges are difficult to establish. It has the power to influence individual mood as well as induce relaxation or stimulate mental or physical performance.

Scientific evidence show that the human brain has systems for music perception which operate from birth, enabling ‘significant nonverbal communication in the form of music’ (Gaston 1968, p.15 as quoted by Hallam p, 4). It is also suggested that participating in music generates social bonding and cultural coherence as well as formation and maintenance of group identity, collective thinking, coalition forming, and promotion of co-operative behavior.

Though there are arguments that music exists simply because of the pleasure that it affords, its basis is purely hedonic, there is no doubt that engagement with it is rewarding for human beings. Even though music has varied roles to play in individual and social development the extent to which music education is provided through state education systems internationally varies.

The approach to formal music education focuses on listening, understanding and appreciation of music, performance, and creativity or it is integrated with general arts education. However, a comprehensive music education approach has not been evolved so far. At present academicians and education professionals express deep concern about the nature, role, importance, and future of arts education in the schools in order to provide quality education to younger generations and make them successful in their future education and career development.

Much of the research results indicate that education in arts provides significant cognitive benefits and boosts academic achievement, beginning at an early age and continuing through school. Music helps to develop cognitive and higher order thinking skills necessary for academic success, as music improves individual talent in the rhythm sense, physical coordination, motor skills, critical thinking, memory recall, listening, and logic development. Research studies have shown that students who listen to music have higher spatial scores, the ability to form mental images of physical objects, on intelligence tests. (Rauseher, et al., 1994)

Among students of arts with music it is found that they learn how to work cooperatively, pose and solve problems, and forge the vital link between individual or group effort and quality of results, which are important for success in a competitive workplace. Well organized arts education contributes to building technological competencies as well as higher level thinking skills of analyzing, synthesizing, and evaluating both personal experience and objective data. In addition, arts education enhances student’s respect for the cultures, belief systems, and values of their fellow learners.

Along with the advancement in scientific techniques, for studying functioning of brain, there is proportionate increase in research exploring the representation of music-related functions of the brain. Altenmuller (2003) states that “the neural systems underlying music appear to be distributed through the left and right cerebral and cerebellar hemispheres, with different aspects of music processed by distinct neural circuits.”(Hallam p.11).

Further researchers opine that the majority of relevant sound information is neurally encoded, even if we are not consciously aware of it, and the attention process of the listener has important implications for the variations of their reaction to music. It is also found that music activates large parts of auditory cortex in both hemispheres of our brain. There is a wide range of music-related behaviors, such as amusia (loss of musical function), aphasia (loss of language functions), etc., which are influenced by brain functions that can be spared or impaired depending on the extent of damage to brain.

Learning occurs by the self-organization of cerebral cortex in response to external stimuli, and learning and memory are based on changes in “synaptic efficacy in the brain.” (Hallam, p.17) The brain network undergoes changes for adapting cortical remodeling, with effective connections between neurons. The brain is able to adapt quickly to environmental demands in the short and long term, and over time develops appropriate neurological structures to meet individual needs. It is evidenced that “the brain responds to behavioral needs but, once developed, enhanced brain functions operate in processing under passive listening conditions, suggesting that they are deeply engrained processing strategies shaped by years of musical experience” (ibid p.19).

LeDoux (1996) suggests that music is capable to activate phylo-genetically old parts of the nervous system that are strongly implicated in the induction of learning of fear responses which operate subconsciously (ibid p.21). When we hear music or other sounds, our emotional responses to them are controlled by amygdale, which provide a more complete cognitive assessment of the situation. In the same pattern when the students are taught about rhythm through verbal, logical explanations accompanied by musical examples, then their approach will be controlled by nature of learning. With adequate time spent in learning, the brain will develop appropriate neurological networks to retain the knowledge and skills learnt.

Research suggests that listening to music while studying may “distract attention from the studied material, thereby impeding learning.” (Tan, L. 1999). Music is every where in the life of an individual, starting from the fetus stage till the end of the life. People listen to different types of music depending on their mood, and the effects of listening to music are personal to each person, which produces different emotion to different people. Any music played while the attention of the listener is focused primarily on a task or activity other than listening to music, is defined as “background music,” and an individual engaged in studying or academic preparation may not be aware of the music in their immediate environment (Radocy & Boyle, 1988).

It may be possible that music enhances some individual’s learning, but it may be distracting to others. Researchers like Radocy & Boyle (1988) have explored the possible transfer of cognitive abilities to other curricular areas by hypothesizing that exposure to music, through participation and formal instruction can facilitate nonmusical learning. With popularization of “Mozart effect” in 1993 by Rauscher et al, claim that listening to Mozart improves intelligence, there is much argument about the potential role of music in developing intelligence, particularly in students.(Listening to music)

Research has found that positive, happy music helps the learner to remember positive facts, whereas negative, sad music helps the learner to remember negative facts, and may even hinder the recall of positive facts. As the brain has to associate the music with an emotion, and when what is being studied is comparable to the music being heard, then the music will help recall the learning. It is common among students to engage in multi-tasking, meaning watching TV, listening to music, surfing the web and chatting online while doing home work.

With multi-tasking students get a superficial understanding of the studied material. As many activities interfere with the studies leading to poor performance, it can have negative impact on learning of students. It is difficult to identify whether passive distraction affect student learning as student’s study preferences vary from individual to individual. For some students’ music functions like a shield from distractions, and for others music can produce emotional soothing.

And for those with attention-deficit disorder, who are constantly seeking stimulation, some distraction may be helpful to concentrate on their studies. If the students are able to identify their potential and they find listening to music makes them more enjoyable to do homework and studying it will be probably a good approach as long as it does not affect their learning.

The constantly changing media and influx of more and more entertainment industry activities are more likely to divide the mind and interrupt studying than what was happening with background music. However, educators need to acknowledge the power that music has to influence moods, emotions, and arousal levels as music stimulates rewards systems in the brain and engagement with it is naturally enjoyable. Through informal engagements with music in a range of social occasions, where children will have extensive exposure to a wide range of musical genres, it will be easy to amalgamate music in the life of students without interfering with their studies.

Works Cited

Rauscher, F., Shaw, G., Levine, L., Ky, K., and Wright, E. Music and Spatial task performance: a casual relationship. Paper presented at the American Psychological Association 102 nd Annual Convention, Los Angeles, CA.

Radocy, R.E., and Boyle, J.D. Psychological Foundations of Musical Behavior (2 nd ed). Springfield: Charles C. Thomas. 1988.

Hallam, Susan. Music Psychology in Education. London: Institute of Education, University of London. 2006. Web.

Tan, L. Effects of distracting noise on study efficacy. Journal of Psychology. 23, (233-226). 1999.

  • Chicago (A-D)
  • Chicago (N-B)

IvyPanda. (2022, April 29). The Effects of Listening to Music While Studying. https://ivypanda.com/essays/the-effects-of-listening-to-music-while-studying/

"The Effects of Listening to Music While Studying." IvyPanda , 29 Apr. 2022, ivypanda.com/essays/the-effects-of-listening-to-music-while-studying/.

IvyPanda . (2022) 'The Effects of Listening to Music While Studying'. 29 April.

IvyPanda . 2022. "The Effects of Listening to Music While Studying." April 29, 2022. https://ivypanda.com/essays/the-effects-of-listening-to-music-while-studying/.

1. IvyPanda . "The Effects of Listening to Music While Studying." April 29, 2022. https://ivypanda.com/essays/the-effects-of-listening-to-music-while-studying/.

Bibliography

IvyPanda . "The Effects of Listening to Music While Studying." April 29, 2022. https://ivypanda.com/essays/the-effects-of-listening-to-music-while-studying/.

  • Classical Music: Influence on Brain and Mood
  • Mindful listening
  • Music Effects on the Brain
  • Concreteness of Words and Free Recall Memory
  • Helpful Listening Skills Review
  • Action Plan for Better Listening
  • Brain and Memory
  • How to Enhance Listening Skills
  • Listening to Children: Why Is It Important?
  • How Music Affects the Brain?
  • Society and Social Policy Analysis
  • Social Construction Model Analysis
  • Cognitive Psychology: Culture and Cognition
  • Cognitive Psychology: Intelligence and Wisdom
  • Deductive Reasoning: Social Contract Algorithm

We use cookies to improve your experience on our site and to show you personalised advertising. To find out more, read our privacy policy and cookie policy

  • Media Centre

A female student studying and listening to headphones

Is it OK to listen to music while studying?

October 17, 2019

UOW researcher answers this tricky question as NSW students start written exams for the HSC.

It’s a good question! In a nutshell, music puts us in a better mood, which makes us better at studying – but it also distracts us, which makes us worse at studying.

So if you want to study effectively with music, you want to reduce how distracting music can be, and increase the level to which the music keeps you in a good mood.

Read more: Curious Kids: Why do adults think video games are bad?

Music can put us in a better mood

You may have heard of the Mozart effect – the idea that listening to Mozart makes you “smarter”. This is based on research that found listening to complex classical music like Mozart improved test scores, which the researcher argued was based on the music’s ability to stimulate parts of our minds that play a role in mathematical ability.

However, further research conclusively debunked the Mozart effect theory: it wasn’t really anything to do with maths, it was really just that music puts us in a better mood.

Research conducted in the 1990s found a “Blur Effect” – where kids who listened to the BritPop band Blur seemed to do better on tests. In fact, researchers found that the Blur effect was bigger than the Mozart effect, simply because kids enjoyed pop music like Blur more than classical music.

Being in a better mood likely means that we try that little bit harder and are willing to stick with challenging tasks.

essay about listening to music while studying

Music can distract us

On the other hand, music can be a distraction – under certain circumstances.

When you study, you’re using your “working memory” – that means you are holding and manipulating several bits of information in your head at once.

The research is fairly clear that when there’s music in the background, and especially music with vocals, our working memory gets worse .

Likely as a result, reading comprehension decreases when people listen to music with lyrics . Music also appears to be more distracting for people who are introverts than for people who are extroverts, perhaps because introverts are more easily overstimulated.

Some clever work by an Australia-based researcher called Bill Thompson and his colleagues aimed to figure out the relative effect of these two competing factors - mood and distraction.

They had participants do a fairly demanding comprehension task, and listen to classical music that was either slow or fast, and which was either soft or loud.

They found the only time there was any real decrease in performance was when people were listening to music that was both fast and loud (that is, at about the speed of Shake It Off by Taylor Swift, at about the volume of a vacuum cleaner).

But while that caused a decrease in performance, it wasn’t actually that big a decrease. And other similar research also failed to find large differences.

essay about listening to music while studying

So… can I listen to music while studying or not?

To sum up: research suggests it’s probably fine to listen to music while you’re studying - with some caveats.

It’s better if:

  • it puts you in a good mood
  • it’s not too fast or too loud
  • it’s less wordy (and hip-hop, where the words are rapped rather than sung, is likely to be even more distracting)
  • you’re not too introverted.

Happy listening and good luck in your exams!

Read more: Curious Kids: Why do old people hate new music?

Timothy Byron , Lecturer in Psychology, University of Wollongong

This article is republished from The Conversation under a Creative Commons license. Read the original article .

UOW academics exercise academic freedom by providing expert commentary, opinion and analysis on a range of ongoing social issues and current affairs. This expert commentary reflects the views of those individual academics and does not necessarily reflect the views or policy positions of the University of Wollongong.

You may also be interested in

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List

Logo of springeropen

The effect of preferred background music on task-focus in sustained attention

Department of Psychology, Goldsmiths University of London, London, UK

Karina J. Linnell

Associated data.

Datasets obtained during, and/or analysed during, the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Although many people listen to music while performing tasks that require sustained attention, the literature is inconclusive about its effects. The present study examined performance on a sustained-attention task and explored the effect of background music on the prevalence of different attentional states, founded on the non-linear relationship between arousal and performance. Forty students completed a variation of the Psychomotor Vigilance Task —that has long been used to measure sustained attention—in silence and with their self-selected or preferred music in the background. We collected subjective reports of attentional state (specifically mind-wandering, task-focus and external distraction states) as well as reaction time (RT) measures of performance. Results indicated that background music increased the proportion of task-focus states by decreasing mind-wandering states but did not affect external distraction states. Task-focus states were linked to shorter RTs than mind-wandering or external distraction states; however, background music did not reduce RT or variability of RT significantly compared to silence. These findings show for the first time that preferred background music can enhance task-focused attentional states on a low-demanding sustained-attention task and are compatible with arousal mediating the relationship between background music and task-performance.

Introduction

Although many people listen to background music during tasks that require sustained attention, there is still no consensus about its effect on performance (for reviews, see Kämpfe, Sedlmeier, & Renkewitz, 2011 ; Küssner 2017 ). Research has focused on background music and sustained attention for decades (e.g., Davies, Lang, & Shackleton, 1973 ) but findings are contradictory, with some studies suggesting a positive influence of the music (e.g., Davies et al., 1973 ; Corhan & Gounard, 1976 ; Fontaine & Schwalm, 1979 ; Turner, Fernandez, & Nelson, 1996 ; Ünal, Waard de, Epstude, & Steg, 2013 ) but others suggesting the opposite (e.g., Brodsky & Slor, 2013 ; Febriandirza, Wu, Ming, Hu, & Zhang, 2017 ; North & Hargreaves, 1999 ; Shih, Huang, & Chiang, 2012 ).

Even though sustained attention is crucial for successful performance (Robertson & O’Connell, 2010 ), sustaining focus on task-relevant information over an extended time period is demanding, leading to time-on-task effects and attentional lapses (Robertson, Manly, Andrade, Baddeley, & Yiend, 1997 ; Unsworth & Robison, 2016 ). Attentional lapses have been found to be underpinned by the locus coeruleus–norepinephrine system (LC–NE; Cohen, Aston-Jones, & Gilzenrat, 2004 ), a neuromodulatory nucleus in the brain stem that projects norepinephrine to the neocortex and mediates effects of arousal (Berridge & Waterhouse, 2003 ). When baseline LC activity, or arousal, is either too low or too high, people perform poorer (Aston-Jones & Cohen, 2005 ) and experience attentional lapses that have been characterised, respectively, as episodes of mind-wandering (hypo-arousal) or external distraction (hyper-arousal) (i.e., off-task attentional states; Unsworth & Robison, 2016 ). Only an intermediate baseline LC activity is linked to optimal performance and a task-focused attentional state (i.e., on-task state; see Fig.  1 ; Aston-Jones & Cohen, 2005 ; Unsworth & Robison, 2016 ).

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is 426_2020_1400_Fig1_HTML.jpg

Inverted-U relationship between arousal, attentional states (mind-wandering, task-focus, external distraction) and performance, as hypothesised by Unsworth and Robison ( 2016 ) and in similar form Yerkes and Dodson ( 1908 )

Understanding the effect of background music on these different arousal-driven on-task and off-task attentional states (see Fig.  1 above) is important for theoretical as well as practical reasons: it is important for improving our understanding both (i) of the real-life situations in which background music listening might be beneficial for attention and performance and also (ii) of core attentional function, and the complex relationship between arousal and attention. To our knowledge, our study is the first that has explored the relationship between background music and the prevalence of different attentional states—both on-task and off-task—on a sustained-attention task. In addition, the fact that it focused on self-selected background music distinguishes it further and gives it ecological validity and contemporary relevance.

As previous research has suggested, arousal may mediate the relationship between background music and performance (Beh & Hirst, 1999 ; Cassidy & MacDonald, 2007 ; North & Hargreaves, 1999 ; Ünal et al., 2013 ; Wang, Jimison, Richard, & Chuan, 2015 ). Specifically, in line with the inverted-U curve linking arousal to performance, previous research has found that background music increases arousal (Burkhard, Elmer, Kara, Brauchli, & Jäncke, 2018 ; Ünal et al., 2013 ) and that, when people are presented with an easy low-arousing task, the music can increase arousal resulting in better performance (Fontaine & Schwalm, 1979 ; Ünal et al., 2013 ). Contrarily, when people are presented with a difficult more arousing task, the music results in poorer performance, compatible with music increasing arousal to too high a level (Beh & Hirst, 1999 ; North & Hargreaves, 1999 ; Wang et al., 2015 ). In the current study, we used an easy low-arousing sustained-attention task—a version of the standard sustained-attention task called the Psychomotor Vigilance Task previously used by Unsworth and Robison ( 2016 )—to imitate the boringness of real-life monotonous tasks and to maximise the potential for music to produce, via arousal, a beneficial effect.

Using self-selected or preferred music in this study, we could manipulate the ‘arousingness’ of the environment in an ecologically valid manner and control for personal preferences (Cassidy & MacDonald, 2009 ; Darrow, Johnson, Agnew, Fuller, & Uchisaka, 2006 ; Ünal, Steg, & Epstude, 2012 , Ünal et al., 2013 ) as well as individual differences in baseline arousal. Because baseline arousal level varies across people and depends on factors, such as personality (e.g., Cassidy & MacDonald, 2007 ; Furnham & Allass, 1999 ; Salame & Baddeley, 1982 ), the ‘arousingness’ and the qualities of the music required to achieve optimal arousal and performance also varies across people. Nevertheless, we conducted exploratory analyses of the impact on sustained attention of qualities of the preferred music stimuli, namely tempo, lyrics and genre, as these have all been shown to impact attention and performance (see, e.g., Amezcua, Guevara, & Ramos-Loyo, 2005 ; Angel, Polzella, & Elvers, 2010 ; Chew, 2010 ; Corhan & Gounard, 1976 ; Darrow et al., 2006 ; Davies et al., 1973 ; Drai-Zerbib & Baccino, 2017 ; Shih et al., 2012 ).

In this study, we aimed to see how preferred background music impacts the distribution of attentional states, as well as the effects of time-on-task on sustained attention. By adopting subjective reports of attentional states, the present study aimed to provide a richer and more informative measure of attention than is available from performance-based measures (Unsworth & Robison, 2016 ). Participants were asked to report their conscious experience at random periods throughout the task. Eliciting these subjective reports allowed us to distinguish poorer performance that resulted from the two types of attentional lapses, mind-wandering (hypo-arousal) and external distraction (hyper-arousal), and to explore the differential effect of background music on these lapses. Furthermore, measuring the percentage of task-focus reports gave an indication of how close to optimal performance was, in a way that focusing only on performance-based measures could not.

In addition to collecting subjective reports of attentional state, the present study examined the impact of background music on time-on-task effects. Background music might exert a positive influence on time-on-task effects via arousal: given that arousal falls off with time-on-task (e.g., Whyte, Polansky, Fleming, Coslett, & Cavallucci, 1995 ), it might need to be boosted by the music over time to reach an optimal level. Alternatively, it could also be the case that any beneficial effects of the music over time would be counteracted by the increasing tendency of participants to habituate to the music (Burkhard et al., 2018 ).

In our first hypothesis, we predicted an interaction effect between attentional-state category (i.e., mind-wandering, task-focus, external distraction) and background music. Specifically, we expected that preferred background music would increase the proportion of task-focus states compared to states of mind-wandering and external distraction. This interaction effect was measured on proportions of subjectively reported attentional states as a function of music-present/absent. We also looked at how the proportion of task-focus reports changes during performance as a measure of time-on-task.

In the second hypothesis, we expected on-task states (task-focus) to be linked to faster reaction times (RTs) than off-task states (mind-wandering, external distraction) to verify that subjective reports are a valid representation of attentional state and express themselves in behaviour (Unsworth & Robison, 2016 ).

Our third hypothesis was based on the first two hypotheses and predicted that if background music is linked to more task-focus states and task-focus is linked to faster and less variable RTs, then we would expect an effect of background music on RTs. Therefore, we hypothesised that mean RTs would be shorter, and increases in RTs with time-on-task would be smaller, with background music than in silence.

The design was a within-subject design; all participants completed the task in silence and with background music. The independent variables were music-present/absent, time-on-task (block 1, 2, 3, 4, and 5) and attentional-state category (mind-wandering, task-focus, external distraction). The order of the music conditions was counterbalanced, meaning that every second, participant completed the task in the same order. This resulted in 20 participants completing the task in music-present followed by music-absent order and 20 participants in music-absent followed by music-present order. One dependent variable was the frequency with which each of the three attentional-state categories was reported, henceforth termed thought-probe response proportions. The other two dependent variables were RT (in milliseconds; ms), and standard deviation of RT, while performing a sustained-attention task. Prior to the main experiment, a pilot study was conducted.

Participants

Participants were students living in and around London, who took part in the study on a voluntary basis in exchange for £5. Only students who normally listen to background music when performing attention-demanding tasks were included in the experiment. This inclusion criterion was supported by findings that people perform better in their preferred listening condition (Crawford & Strapp, 1994 ; Nantais & Schellenberg, 1999 ). There were 40 participants in total. The sample size was based on a power calculation showing that a minimum sample of 34 people would be sufficient for a medium (0.25) effect size with a 0.80 power level and 0.05 alpha level.

Participants included 23 females and 17 males between the ages of 19 and 32 ( M  = 24, SD = 3.33). Analysis of gender did not show any significant effects on thought-probe response proportions or on overall RT measures of performance, nor a significant interaction with music-present/absent on these measures on the sustained-attention task. Thus, gender is not included in further analyses of these measures. This null result agrees with past research suggesting that there is no effect of gender on sustained attention (e.g., Chan, 2009 ).

27 of the participants had received previous formal musical training (voice or instrument) in a music school or private setting ( M  = 6 years; Min. = 2 years; Max. = 21 years). Musical training was measured by asking participants whether they had had any previous musical training and if so, for how many years. Analysis of musical training did not show any significant effects on thought-probe response proportions or on overall RT measures of performance, nor a significant interaction with music-present/absent on these measures. Therefore, musical training is not analysed further here. Indeed, although there are previous studies that have shown an effect of musical training on sustained attention, they have not shown an interaction between training and the effect of background music (e.g., Darrow et al., 2006 ; Rodrigues, Loureiro, & Caramelli, 2013 ).

Pilot study

A pilot study was conducted to refine the thought-probe stimuli (see “ Materials” ). A different set of ten participants completed the pilot study, seven females and three males, who were all students at London-based universities.

Preferred background music

Participants were asked to send a 30-min long playlist containing their preferred background music tracks to the experimenter prior to participation in the study. There were no restrictions on the music, but participants were asked to send a playlist they would normally listen to when performing an attention-demanding task. The individual tracks could be of any length (mean track duration was 215.57 s) but together the playlist had to cover the full 30 min of the music-present session of the experiment. A few playlists were shorter than 30 min, because some participants decided to listen to their chosen tracks in repeat, as they would normally do in real-life settings. Musicological data for each track were collected from the database of the digital music streaming service called Spotify, using the Spotify Web API endpoints (Spotify 2018 ). This included data on tempo (overall tempo of a track in beats/minute; M  = 112.56 BPM, SD = 32.37 BPM, range for individual tracks = 52.16–215.04 BPM), lyrics (i.e., a measure between 0.00 and 1.00 indicating the likelihood of a track containing any vocals at any given point during the track; tracks with a lower lyrics level were less likely to contain any vocal content; M  = 0.59, SD  = 0.57, range for individual tracks = 0.01–1.00), and genre. Genre categorisation for each track was based on Spotify’s main genre categories (Spotify 2018 ); because there was no specific genre categorisation available for individual tracks, genres describing the artist and the album in which the track appeared were used. Specifically, the most frequently occurring main genre was chosen for each track. Tracks that only had subgenre labels and not a clear main genre assigned to them were placed in the ‘other’ category.

Although some previous studies have shown that music preferences are influenced by gender, suggesting that males prefer “heavier” (Colley, 2008 ), more “vigorous” (Soares-Quadros Júnior, Lorenzo, Herrera, & Araújo Santos, 2019 ), and “intense-rebellious” music (Dobrota, Reić Ercegovac, & Habe, 2019 ) compared to females, in this study, there was no effect of gender on preferences for either tempo, lyrics, or genre.

Sustained attention task

The sustained-attention task was a variation of the Psychomotor Vigilance Task developed by Dinges and Powell ( 1985 ), which has long been used to measure sustained attention (Unsworth & Robison, 2016 ). As shown in Fig.  2 , participants were first presented with a fixation cross in the middle of the screen on a grey background for 2 s (s). Then, they saw a clock without any numbers, specifically a black circle containing a clock-hand at the 12-o’clock position and, after a variable wait time (equally distributed from 2 to 10 s in 500 ms increments), the clock-hand started moving clockwise in a smooth analogue fashion. The analogue clock was developed instead of the digital one used in previous experiments to make the task both applicable with populations with different/no number systems and also lower in cognitive demand. The task of the participants was to press the left mouse button as quickly as possible once the hand of the clock started moving. After the participant had pressed the mouse button, the clock remained on the screen for 1 s to provide feedback. Then, a 500-ms blank screen was presented, followed by either the next trial or a thought-probe. Participants completed 5 blocks, with 34 trials in each block, for both the music and no-music conditions. One block lasted approximately 6 min. Before starting the experiment, participants completed five practice trials to become familiar with the task.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is 426_2020_1400_Fig2_HTML.jpg

Schematic representation of a single experimental trial with a thought-probe after the trial

During each block, participants were periodically presented with thought-probes. They were primed to respond to these probes by providing subjective reports to classify their immediately preceding thoughts. In all, they were presented with six thought-probes per block, after six randomly selected trials.

Thought-probes were refined in the pilot study based on participants’ feedback. Data obtained in the pilot study phase were not analysed or reported. Thought-probe statements, as specified below, were based on those used by Unsworth and Robison ( 2016 ). The original statements that were used by Unsworth and Robison ( 2016 ) are presented below:

“Please characterise your current conscious experience.

  • I am totally focused on the current task.
  • I am thinking about my performance on the task or how long it is taking.
  • I am distracted by sights/sounds/temperature or by physical sensations (hungry/thirsty).
  • I am daydreaming/my mind is wandering about things unrelated to the task.
  • I am not very alert/my mind is blank or I’m drowsy.”

Some alterations were made compared to previous research based on feedback from the participants in the pilot study, to avoid ambiguous expressions that could refer to different concepts depending on one’s cultural background and understanding. More specifically, compared to the original five statements, only three statements were included in this study, namely those referring to mind-wandering, on-task thoughts/task-focus and external distraction, respectively (see the statements below). The term mind-wandering was left out of the phrasing of statement 1, and statements 2 and 3—referring to task-focus and external distraction—were simplified.

  • I am tired, my mind is blank, or my thoughts are elsewhere.
  • I am focused on the task or how I am doing it.
  • I am thinking about the things around me (people, sights, sounds, the temperature) or about sensations in my body (hunger, thirst, pain).”

Apparatus and procedure

The present study was approved by the Psychology Department Ethics Committee at Goldsmiths, University of London on the 6th of June 2018. Participants were tested individually in the lab, in a dark and quiet room, and stimuli were presented on a 24-in. monitor with a 1920 × 1080 screen resolution.

Participants were asked not to do any heavy exercise or drink any caffeinated drinks 2–3 h prior to participation and to have a good night’s sleep involving at least 7–8 h sleep the night before. Prior to the day of the study, participants sent a 30-min long playlist with their preferred background tracks to the researcher. Background music was played continuously throughout the task in the music condition from a mobile device in “do not disturb” mode to avoid any distractions. Participants could either use their own headphones or a pair provided by the researcher to listen to the music. For the music-absent condition, the headphones were removed to increase ecological validity (given that participants would not normally wear headphones when completing a task in silence).

Upon first arrival in the lab at the start of the experiment, participants first read the information sheet and signed the consent form. Then, they received the task instructions and were presented with five practice trials before the first block in both the music-present and music-absent conditions. They had the opportunity to ask any questions throughout the practice trials.

The instructions they received for the practice trials also applied to the main task. First participants were instructed to look at the fixation cross in the centre of the screen before each trial. Then, it was explained to them that their tasks were (i) to stop the clock-hand in the clock-face in the middle of the screen as soon as the clock-hand started moving, by pressing the left mouse button; (ii) when presented with thought-probe statements after some of the trials, to choose the statement that best described their current conscious experience by pressing buttons 1, 2, or 3 on the keyboard.

The study took approximately 1 h and 30 min, during which time participants performed the sustained-attention task twice: once in silence (30 min) and once with their preferred music playing in the background (30 min). There were five blocks of trials in each condition. Each block was started individually by the experimenter, ensuring that there was a break for a few minutes between the blocks so that participants could rest and drink some water if they wished to. To control for carry-over effects of the music conditions, music conditions were counterbalanced and there was a 10-min break between the conditions in both orders. During the break, participants used a tablet to play a word spelling game called ‘Hi Words’ that was unrelated to the study. The game kept them engaged and helped them recover from the fatigue caused by the first condition (e.g., Jahncke, Hygge, Halin, Green, & Dimberg, 2011 ).

Once participants had finished the task, the researcher recorded their age, gender and whether they had had any previous formal music training and, if yes, for how many years. Then, the participants received the debriefing sheet and £5 for their participation.

Analyses were conducted on the effect of preferred background music both on the subjective measures of attention and behavioural measures of performance. The independent variables were music-present/absent, time-on-task (blocks 1, 2, 3, 4, and 5) and attentional-state category (mind-wandering, task-focus, external distraction). For the dependent variables, following Unsworth and Robison ( 2016 ), we examined: thought-probe response proportions of attentional-state category, mean RT (in ms) and standard deviation of RT (in ms). For the two RT analyses, some data points were removed, including any number below 100 ms (18 data points) based on Basner and Dinges’ ( 2011 ) suggestion that RTs below 100 ms count as false starts (errors of commission); moreover, any number above 5000 ms that exceeded the time required for the clock-hand to complete a full revolution was also removed (two data points).

Order of the music conditions (music-absent followed by music-present, or music-present followed by music-absent) did not show any significant effects on thought-probe response proportions, RT, or standard deviation of RT ( p  ≥ 0.23), and there was no interaction between order and music-present/absent on these variables ( p  ≥ 0.10); therefore, analyses were collapsed across order.

In all, four different analyses were conducted on thought-probe response proportions, RT, and standard deviation of RT. In the first analysis, a three-way repeated-measures ANOVA was conducted to examine the effect of attentional-state category, music-present/absent and time-on-task on thought-probe response proportions. In the second analysis, RT was analysed; however, based on Unsworth and Robison ( 2016 ), we anticipated having insufficient data to perform the corresponding three-way ANOVA. In other words, we anticipated having too many empty cells for RT response for trials associated with thought-probe statements as a consequence of participants not all reporting all attentional-state categories. Therefore, a separate linear-mixed-model analysis was used, which accounted for unequal sample sizes, to explore the effect of attentional-state category on RT as a check that the subjective reports are meaningful and express themselves in behaviour. The third analysis also focused on RT: it was a two-way repeated-measures ANOVA and was performed to analyse the effect of music presence/absence and time-on-task on RT. Finally, the fourth analysis focused on the standard deviation of RT: like the third, it was a two-way repeated-measures ANOVA conducted on the effect of music-present/absent and time-on-task, but this time on the standard deviation of RT.

At the end of the Results section, we report additional analyses on tempo, lyrics, and genre. These additional analyses should be treated as exploratory given that participants were free to select tracks that varied in tempo, lyrics, and genre. As a result, determining the values of these musicological parameters for each participant involved factoring in the proportion of time that the participant spent listening to tracks of varying tempo, lyrics, and genre.

The effect of music-present/absent, attentional-sate category, and time-on-task on thought-probe response proportions

To analyse the effect of background music on thought-probe response proportions, a 2 × 3 × 5 repeated-measures factorial ANOVA was conducted on music-present/absent, attentional-state category and time-on-task. Greenhouse–Geisser correction was used, because the sphericity assumption was violated, p  < 0.02. There was a significant main effect of attentional-state category, F (1.66, 64.98) = 35.06, MSE = 21.64, p  < 0.001, partial η 2  = 0.47, and a significant quadratic trend, F (1, 39) = 50.97, MSE = 35.98, p  < 0.001, partial η 2  = 0.57. This shows that participants reported significantly more task-focus states ( M  = 0.58, SD = 0.33) than mind-wandering ( M  = 0.22, SD = 0.28) or external distraction states ( M  = 0.20, SD = 0.24).

Importantly, there was also a significant interaction between music-present/absent and attentional-state category, F (1.45, 56.37) = 4.03, MSE = 0.98, p  = 0.04, partial η 2  = 0.09, and a significant linear trend, F (1, 39) = 6.44, MSE = 0.46, p  = 0.02, partial η 2  = 0.14. The simple effect analysis (paired-sample t tests) showed a significant difference between music-present and music-absent conditions in the proportion of both mind-wandering, t (199) = 4.24, p  < 0.001, d  = 0.30, and task-focus reports, t (199) = − 2.82, p  = 0.005, d  = 0.20, and a non-significant difference in external distraction reports, ( p  = 0.72, d  = 0.03). Specifically, there were significantly more mind-wandering reports in the music-absent condition ( M  = 0.27, SD = 0.28) than in the music-present condition ( M  = 0.18, SD = 0.28) and significantly more task-focus reports in the music-present condition ( M  = 0.62, SD = 0.34) than in the music-absent condition ( M  = 0.54, SD = 0.33), as seen in Fig.  3 .

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is 426_2020_1400_Fig3_HTML.jpg

Thought-probe response proportions as a function of attentional-state category and music-present/absent (music/no music). Error bars represent ± 1 standard error of the mean (S.E.M.)

Furthermore, the ANOVA also revealed a significant interaction between attentional-state category and time-on-task, F (5.00, 195.13) = 8.23, MSE = 0.90, p  < 0.001, partial η 2  = 0.17, and a significant linear, F (1, 39) = 5.81, MSE = 0.55, p  = 0.02, partial η 2  = 0.13, and quadratic trend, F (1, 39) = 25.05, MSE  = 3.87, p  < 0.001, partial η 2  = 0.39 (see Fig.  4 ).

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is 426_2020_1400_Fig4_HTML.jpg

Thought-probe response proportions as a function of time-on-task (block number). Error bars represent ± 1 S.E.M

The simple effect analysis (using paired-sample t tests with Bonferroni correction) on mind-wandering reports and time-on-task showed a significant difference in the proportion of mind-wandering reports between blocks 1–3 ( p  = 0.003, d  = 0.49), 1–4 ( p  < 0.001, d  = 0.72), 1–5 ( p  < 0.001, d  = 0.62), 2–4 ( p  = 0.001, d  = 0.57), and 2–5 ( p  = 0.006, d  = 0.46). A simple regression analysis demonstrated that, as block number increased, the proportion of mind-wandering reports also increased, F (1, 3) = 192.00, p  = 0.001, R 2  = 0.99.

Moreover, the simple effect analysis (using paired-sample t tests with Bonferroni correction) on task-focus reports and time-on-task also showed a significant difference in the proportion of task-focus reports between blocks 1–3 ( p  = 0.007, d  = 0.45), 1–4 ( p  < 0.001, d  = 0.71), 1–5 ( p  < 0.001, d  = 0.70), 2–4 ( p  < 0.001, d  = 0.60), 2–5 ( p  < 0.001, d  = 0.64), 3–4 ( p  = 0.004, d  = 0.48) and 3–5 ( p  = 0.005, d  = 0.47). A simple regression analysis demonstrated that, as block number increased, the proportion of task-focus reports decreased, F (1, 3) = 294.00, p  < 0.001, R 2  = 0.99.

The simple effect analysis (paired-sample t tests) on external distraction reports and time-on-task did not show any significant difference in the proportion of external distraction reports between blocks ( p  ≥ 0.23, d  ≤ 0.19).

Finally, the ANOVA revealed that there was not a significant 3-way interaction between music-present/absent, attentional-state category and time-on-task (p  = 0.13).

The effect of attentional-sate category on reaction time

Because not all participants reported all of the attentional states, there were too many missing cases to perform a corresponding (three-way ANOVA) analysis on RT. Therefore, a separate linear-mixed-model analysis was performed on attentional-state category and RT, followed by a two-way ANOVA on the effect on RT of time-on-task and music-present/absent. Finally, a similar two-way ANOVA was performed on the effect of time-on-task and music-present/absent on the standard deviation of RT.

First, the relationship between attentional-state category and RT was examined to explore the correlation between subjective and behavioural measures. We expected that on-task (task-focus) states would be linked to faster reaction time than off-task states (mind-wandering and external distraction). Because four participants did not report mind-wandering and sample sizes were unequal (40 participants in the task-focus group, 40 in the external-distraction group, and 36 participants in the mind-wandering group), we performed linear-mixed-model analysis following Unsworth and Robison ( 2016 ). In the model, attentional-state category was entered as a fixed factor and subjects were entered as random factors. The linear-mixed-model analysis suggested that on-task reports were associated with significantly shorter RTs than mind-wandering reports, t (35.48) = 3.89, p  < 0.001 ( b  = 29.61, SE = 13.41), and also with significantly shorter RTs than external distraction reports, t (39) =  − 4.39, p  < 0.001 ( b  = 21.71, SE = 20.68) (see Fig.  5 ).

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is 426_2020_1400_Fig5_HTML.jpg

Mean RT as a function of attentional-state category. Error bars represent ± 1 S.E.M

The Effect of music-present/absent and time-on-task on reaction time

Next, a 5 × 2 repeated-measures ANOVA was conducted on the effect of music-present/absent and time-on-task on RT. Greenhouse–Geisser correction was used for the effect of time-on-task, because the sphericity assumption was violated, p  < 0.001. There was a significant main effect of time-on-task, F (1.44, 56.06) = 7.64, MSE = 17,793.34, p  = 0.003, partial η 2  =  0.1 6, and a significant linear trend of time-on-task, F (1, 39) = 9.90, MSE = 19,464.16, p  = 0.003, partial η 2  =  0.2 0. This means that RT became significantly slower as block number increased (see Fig.  6 ).

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is 426_2020_1400_Fig6_HTML.jpg

Mean RT as a function of time-on-task (block number) and music-present/absent (music/no music). Error bars represent ± 1 S.E.M

The ANOVA did not show a significant main effect of music-present/absent ( p  = 0.12), nor a significant interaction between music-present/absent and time-on-task ( p  = 0.46).

The Effect of music-present/absent and time-on-task on standard deviation of RT

In addition to analysing RTs, the standard deviations of RTs were also analysed in a separate 5 × 2 repeated-measures ANOVA on music-present/absent and time-on-task. Greenhouse–Geisser correction was used for the effect of time-on-task, because the sphericity assumption was violated p  < 0.001. There was a significant main effect of time-on-task, F (2.01, 78.45) = 4.47, MSE = 19,177.63, p  = 0.01, partial η 2  =  0.1 0, and a significant linear trend of time-on-task, F (1, 39) = 7.20, MSE = 164,885.83, p  = 0.01, partial η 2  =  0.1 6. This suggests that not only did participants get slower across blocks, but they also became more variable in their responding as block number increased (see Fig.  7 ).

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is 426_2020_1400_Fig7_HTML.jpg

Standard deviation of RT as a function of time-on-task (block number) and music-present/absent (music/no music). Error bars represent ± 1 S.E.M

The ANOVA did again not show a significant main effect of music-present/absent ( p  = 0.07), nor a significant interaction between music-present/absent and time-on-task ( p  = 0.17).

The Effect of music tempo, lyrics, and genre on thought-probe response proportions, RT, and standard deviation of RT

The effects of tempo, lyrics, and genre were analysed on thought-probe response proportions, on mean RT, and on standard deviation of RT. Tempo was determined for each participant using the average value across tracks, weighted by the duration of each track ( M  = 115.90 BPM, SD = 14.70 BPM, range = 91.32–160.02 BPM). Similarly, lyrics were determined for each participant using the average value across tracks, weighted by the duration of each track ( M  = 0.64, SD = 0.36, range 0.07–1.00). For genre, each participant was assigned to the genre that made up the greatest proportion of time in their playlist (Fig.  8 ).

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is 426_2020_1400_Fig8_HTML.jpg

Each participant was assigned to the genre that made up the greatest proportion of time in their playlist. The graph represents the number of participants for each genre who listened to that genre for the greatest proportion of time. The ‘other’ category included tracks that could not be otherwise categorised, such as Japanese anime music, music for meditation, and some indie and folk tracks that did not clearly fit into a different main category. The ‘instrumental’ category included tracks that were primarily produced using musical instruments, such as remixes of original tracks, piano ‘lounge’ music, and ‘ambient’ music

The sample size for lyrics was 36 because the chosen playlists from four participants were not available on Spotify and, therefore, could not be analysed. The sample size for analyses of tempo was 35, because there was one outlier (with an average tempo of 160.02 BPM) which was excluded from the analyses. The sample size for the analyses of genre was 22, because we compared only the two most popular genres, operationalised as those that were listened to for the greatest proportion of time by the most participants (pop and instrumental).

Five separate simple regression analyses were conducted to see whether tempo can predict (i) proportions of mind-wandering reports, (ii) proportions of task-focus reports, (iii) proportions of external distraction reports, (iv) mean RT scores, and (v) standard deviation of RT. The regression models did not show any association between tempo and mind-wandering states ( p  = 0.66), task-focus states ( p  = 0.32), or external distraction states ( p  = 0.36). Moreover, the regression models also did not show any association between tempo and mean RT ( p  = 0.21), or tempo and standard deviation of RT ( p  = 0.30).

Five separate simple regression analyses were conducted to see whether lyrics can predict (i) proportions of mind-wandering reports, (ii) proportions of task-focus reports, (iii) proportions of external distraction reports, (iv) mean RT scores, and (v) standard deviation of RT. The regression models did not show any association between lyrics and mind-wandering states ( p  = 0.15), task-focus states ( p  = 0.38), or external distraction states ( p  = 0.69). Moreover, the regression models also did not show any association between lyrics and mean RT ( p  = 0.98), or lyrics and standard deviation of RT ( p  = 0.08).

Five separate Mann–Whitney U non-parametric tests were conducted to see whether genre (pop, instrumental) affects (i) proportions of mind-wandering reports, (ii) proportions of task-focus reports, (iii) proportions of external distraction reports, (iv) mean RT scores, and (v) standard deviation of RT. These non-parametric tests were chosen instead of independent t tests because of the unequal sample sizes: there were 17 participants in the pop, and 5 in the instrumental genre category. The Mann–Whitney U tests did not show any effect of genre on mind-wandering reports ( p  = 0.16), task-focus reports ( p  = 0.06), or external distraction reports ( p  = 0.25). Moreover, there was no effect of genre on RT ( p  = 0.88) or standard deviation of RT ( p  = 0.14).

The goal of the present study was to explore how background music affects task-performance and attentional state on a simple sustained-attention task. This study was the first that has used subjective reports of attentional state (representative of the different regions of the inverted-U curve linking arousal to performance; Unsworth & Robison, 2016 ) to explore the effect of background music. The results provided evidence for our first and second hypotheses in which we predicted that background music would increase the proportions of task-focus states compared to silence, and on-task states (i.e., task-focus) would be linked to shorter RTs than off-task states (i.e., mind-wandering, external distraction). However, the third hypothesis was not supported by the results and background music did not affect RT.

As predicted in the first hypothesis, background music was shown to increase task-focus states compared to silence. Specifically, it was found that, while mind-wandering reports decreased, task-focus reports increased when music was playing in the background. Interestingly, all the increase in task-focus could be accounted for by the decrease in mind-wandering, and music did not affect external distraction. In addition, task-focus reports significantly decreased, and mind-wandering reports significantly increased, with time-on-task, suggesting that people were less able to sustain their attention on the task over time and their arousal dropped. These results are likely to be a consequence of the boringness of the sustained-attention task used in the current study, with the result that it made participants hypo-aroused, placing them at the lower end of the arousal axis on the inverted-U curve. As a consequence, the arousal-increase caused by the music led to an intermediate level of arousal, shifting the balance between task-focus and mind-wandering states, and not impacting external distraction states.

This is in line with explanations that the less complex the task, the more positive the influence of music can be expected to be (Anderson 1994 ; Gellatly & Meyer, 1992 ; North & Hargreaves, 1999 ). For example, the present finding is supported by previous research showing that performance is better with background music compared to silence on simple tasks (Ünal et al., 2013 ). Specifically, Ünal et al. ( 2013 ) used a monotonous lane-keeping task and showed that preferred background music increased heart rate and some aspects of driving performance (response latencies to changes in speed on the road, lateral control) compared to silence. Similarly, Fontaine and Schwalm ( 1979 ) found that, compared to unfamiliar background music and silence, familiar music increased participants’ heart rate and correct detections on a simple vigilance task.

Familiar background music has also been linked to less frequent mind-wandering episodes and faster RTs than unfamiliar music by Feng and Bidelman ( 2015 ). They explored how familiar classical and unfamiliar classical music in the background affects mind-wandering on a lexical congruity task and found that familiar music was associated with faster response times and less frequent mind-wandering episodes than unfamiliar music. Feng and Bidelman ( 2015 ) suggested that the more positive effect of familiar music could have been a result of its familiarity increasing emotional arousal and pleasure, and in turn decreasing stress. However, they did not compare the effect of music to silence, nor did they focus on a sustained-attention task (Feng & Bidelman, 2015 ). Because preferred music is almost always familiar to the listener, these previous results showing a positive effect of familiar background music support the potential of preferred background music to enhance task-focused attention, as found in this study.

These past findings and the present results are compatible with the idea that the effect of background music is mediated by arousal. This idea has been raised by North and Hargreaves ( 1999 ), who discussed two possible frameworks—the arousal framework and the cognitive-load framework—that could potentially explain the effect of background music on performance. In terms of the arousal framework already outlined above, when presented with a very simple task, background music should increase arousal to an optimal/intermediate level and, thus, increase performance. Contrarily, in terms of the cognitive-load framework, if the music takes up cognitive space, then it should impair performance even on very simple tasks because it decreases the cognitive space available for the task. The current study provided evidence for the arousal framework because it showed that background music improved performance, specifically by increasing the ratio of task-focus states compared to mind-wandering states which is compatible with an increase in arousal to levels that are optimal for performance.

The results presented here also provided evidence for our second hypothesis, which predicted that states of task-focus will be linked to shorter RTs than off-task states of mind-wandering and external distraction. Therefore, our study was successful in replicating the result by Unsworth and Robison ( 2016 ) showing that on-task states are associated with shorter RTs than off-task states. This result shows that there is a link between subjective and behavioural measures of attentional state and that subjective reports provide valid, meaningful measures of attentional state that express themselves in behaviour.

Our third hypothesis was the only one not supported by the results of the main analyses: background music did not exert effects on either RT or variability of RT. In other words, even though music enhanced task-focus and task-focus was linked to shorter RT, music did not produce an overall significant reduction in RT compared to silence. The predicted effects of music on RT time-on-task effects were also not present. This was despite RT and variability in responding increasing with time-on-task—along with task-focus reports decreasing and mind-wandering reports increasing—indicating that people did become fatigued (Unsworth & Robison, 2016 ). The absence of any effect in the present study of background music on time-on-task effects—either on subjective report or RT data—suggests that background music does not make a difference to how fatigued people become. These findings are consistent with a study by Burkhard et al. ( 2018 ) that found that background music increased arousal but did not affect performance on a sustained-attention task.

Moreover, analysis of tempo, lyrics, and genre in the present study revealed no significant effects on RT, variability of RT, or indeed on thought-probe response proportions. These null results represent a failure to replicate past studies on tempo (e.g., Amezcua et al., 2005 ; Angel et al., 2010 ), lyrics (e.g., Darrow et al., 2006 ; Drai-Zerbib and Baccino, 2017 ; Shih et al., 2012 ) and on genre (e.g., Chew, 2010 ; Corhan and Gounard, 1976 ; Davies et al., 1973 ). The absence of any significant effect of musical parameters (whether tempo, lyrics, or genre) may be a by-product of the use of self-selected music in the current study, making it bound to represent a less satisfactory test of their role than studies directly manipulating these musical parameters within the individual. However, it is entirely possible that participants selected their background music to titrate their arousal to optimal levels, such that participants whose baseline arousal level was low selected more arousing music, while those whose baseline arousal level was high selected less arousing music; in this case, effects of musical parameters mediated by arousal would not be manifested. Because of the absence of an effect of tempo, lyrics, or genre in the current study, and the limited number of studies on these musicological parameters and sustained attention, future research could further explore whether these parameters exert significant effects when listening preferences are controlled for.

In summary, the current study demonstrated that preferred background music enhanced task-focus on a low-demanding sustained-attention task by decreasing mind-wandering; music did not affect external distraction at all. Furthermore, while on-task states were linked to shorter RT than off-task states, background music did not affect RT or variability of RT. Overall, these results provide evidence for a positive effect of background music on task-focused attention during an easy, low-demanding task. Future research should manipulate task-difficulty by including a difficult as well as an easy task. This should permit examination of the full range of the inverted-U curve linking arousal to performance and subjective attentional state, including both types of attentional lapses (mind-wandering and external distraction). The types of attentional lapses could also be broken down into more sub-types (e.g., to include task-relevant distractions, or more types of mind-wandering; see Unsworth & Robison, 2018 ) in future studies to advance our understanding of sub-optimal attentional states and the effect background music has on them. Moreover, to test the generalisability of our results, future experiments should include people who do not normally listen to background music. It would also be interesting to test whether our findings can be replicated when keeping the device for presentation of the sound stimuli (i.e., headphones) consistent across music-present and music-absent conditions.

Listening to background music while performing tasks that require sustained attention is common, although previous research on the benefit of such music listening is inconsistent. The current study aimed to explore how the effects of preferred background music are mediated via arousal and how music affects attentional state. Using preferred or self-selected background music in the study increased ecological validity compared to the many studies investigating background music using researcher-selected music and also allowed us to take listening preferences and differences in baseline arousal levels into account. The present research is the first to investigate the effect of preferred background music on attentional states by collecting subjective reports as well as behavioural measures of performance (RT). The subjective reports employed here are founded on the inverted-U relationship between arousal and performance and can identify attentional lapses (i.e., mind-wandering, external distraction) as well as optimal states (i.e., task-focus), providing a richer and more informative index of attention than simple performance measures (RT). Importantly, listening to preferred background music was found to increase task-focus and decrease mind-wandering states on a low-demanding sustained-attention task although it did not affect overall RT. The increase in task-focus states provides evidence for music’s ability to improve focused attention and performance—by increasing arousal to an intermediate level optimal for performance—and suggests that people can derive benefit from music listening while performing low-demanding tasks. However, preferred background music did not affect time-on-task effects, which suggests that listening to music did not influence how fatigued people became over time.

Acknowledgements

The authors would particularly like to acknowledge the generosity of Nash Unsworth, who very kindly shared his code for the Psychomotor Vigilance Task used in Unsworth and Robison (2016) that inspired the paradigm used in this study. The authors would also like to thank Zeeshan Syed and Bence Szikora for providing valuable technical support and expertise.

Data availability

Compliance with ethical standards.

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee (Psychology Department Ethics Committee at Goldsmiths, University of London, 06/06/2018) and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

  • Amezcua C, Guevara MA, Ramos-Loyo J. Effects of musical tempi on visual attention erps. International Journal of Neuroscience. 2005; 115 (2):193–206. doi: 10.1080/00207450590519094. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Anderson KJ. Impulsitivity, caffeine, and task difficulty: A within-subjects test of the Yerkes-Dodson law. Personality and Individual Differences. 1994; 16 (6):813–829. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(94)90226-7. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Angel LA, Polzella DJ, Elvers GC. Background music and cognitive performance. Perceptual and Motor Skills. 2010; 110 (3):1059–1064. doi: 10.2466/pms.110.C.1059-1064. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Aston-Jones G, Cohen JD. An integrative theory of locus coeruleus-norepinephrine function: Adaptive gain and optimal performance. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2005; 28 :403–450. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.28.061604.135709. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Basner M, Dinges DF. Maximizing sensitivity of the Psychomotor Vigilance Test (PVT) to sleep loss. Sleep. 2011; 34 (5):581–591. doi: 10.1093/sleep/34.5.581. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Beh HC, Hirst R. Performance on driving-related tasks during music. Ergonomics. 1999; 42 (8):1087–1098. doi: 10.1080/001401399185153. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Berridge CW, Waterhouse BD. The locus coeruleus- noradrenergic system: Modulation of behavioral state and state-dependent cognitive processes. Brain Research Reviews. 2003; 42 :33–84. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0173(03)00143-7. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Brodsky W, Slor Z. Background music as a risk factor for distraction among young-novice drivers. Accident Analysis and Prevention. 2013; 59 :382–393. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2013.06.022. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Burkhard A, Elmer S, Kara D, Brauchli C, Jäncke L. The effect of background music on inhibitory functions: An ERP study. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2018 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00293. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cassidy G, MacDonald RAR. The effect of background music and background noise on the task performance of introverts and extraverts. Psychology of Music. 2007; 35 (3):517–537. doi: 10.1177/0305735607076444. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cassidy G, MacDonald R. The effects of music choice on task performance: A study of the impact of self-selected and experimenter-selected music on driving game performance and experience. Musicae Scientiae. 2009; 13 (2):357–386. doi: 10.1177/102986490901300207. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Chan RCK. A further study on the sustained attention response to task (SART): The effect of age, gender and education. Brain Injury. 2009; 15 (9):819–829. doi: 10.1080/02699050110034325. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Chew, C. (2010). Using EEG recordings to examine the relationships between sustained attention and types of background music in individuals with ADHD. In A. J. Spink, F. Grieco, O. E. Krips, L. W. S. Loijens, L. P. J. J. Noldus, & P. H. Zimmerman (ed.), Proceedings of Measuring Behavior 2010, 7th international conference on methods and techniques in behavioral research (pp. 52–54). Eindhoven: Noldus Information Technology.
  • Cohen, J. D., Aston-Jones, G., & Gilzenrat, M. S. (2004). A systems-level perspective on attention and cognitive control: Guided activation, adaptive gating, conflict monitoring, and exploitation vs. exploration. In M. I. Posner (Ed.), Cognitive neuroscience of attention (pp. 71–90). New York: Guilford Press.
  • Colley A. Young people's musical taste: Relationship with gender and gender-related traits. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 2008; 38 (8):2039–2055. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2008.00379.x. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Corhan CM, Gounard BR. Types of music, schedules of background stimulation, and visual vigilance performance. Perceptual and Motor Skills. 1976 doi: 10.2466/pms.1976.42.2.662. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Crawford HJ, Strapp CM. Effects of vocal and instrumental music on visuospatial and verban performance moderated by studying preference and personality. Personality and Individual Differences. 1994; 16 (2):237–245. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(94)90162-7. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Darrow AA, Johnson C, Agnew S, Fuller ER, Uchisaka M. Effect of preferred music as a distraction in music majors’ and nonmusic majors’ selective attention. Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education. 2006; 170 :21–31. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Davies DR, Lang L, Shackleton VJ. The effects of music and task difficulty on performance at a visual vigilance task. British Journal of Psychology. 1973; 64 (3):383–389. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1973.tb01364.x. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dinges DF, Powell JW. Mictrocomputer analyses of performance on a portable, simple visual RT task during sustained operations. Behavior Research Methods Instruments & Computers. 1985; 17 (6):652–655. doi: 10.3758/bf03200977. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dobrota S, Reić Ercegovac I, Habe K. Gender differences in musical taste: The mediating role of functions of music. Drustvena Istrazivanja. 2019; 28 (4):567–586. doi: 10.5559/di.28.4.01. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Drai-Zerbib V, Baccino T. Online effects of musical environment on text reading: Eye-tracking investigation. Psychologie Francaise. 2017; 62 (3):233–247. doi: 10.1016/j.psfr.2014.12.002. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Febriandirza A, Wu CZ, Ming Z, Hu ZZ, Zhang H. The effect of natural sounds and music on driving performance and physiological. Engineering Letters. 2017; 25 (4):455–463. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Feng, S. & Bidelman, G. M. (2015). Music listening and song familiarity modulate mind wandering and behavioral success during lexical processing. In Proceedings of the 37th annual meeting of the cognitive science society (CogSci, 2015) , Pasadena, CA, July 22–25, 2015.
  • Fontaine CW, Schwalm ND. Effects of familiarity of music on vigilant performance. Perceptual and Motor Skills. 1979; 49 (1):71–74. doi: 10.2466/pms.1979.49.1.71. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Furnham A, Allass K. The influence of musical distraction of varying complexity on the cognitive performance of extroverts and introverts. European Journal of Personality. 1999; 13 (1):27–38. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1099-0984(199901/02)13:1<27::aid-per318>3.0.co;2-r. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gellatly IR, Meyer JJ. The effects of goal difficulty on physiological arousal, cognition, and task performance. Journal of Applied Psychology. 1992; 77 (5):694–704. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.77.5.694. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jahncke H, Hygge S, Halin N, Green AM, Dimberg K. Open-plan office noise: Cognitive performance and restoration. Journal of Environmental Psychology. 2011; 31 (4):373–382. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2011.07.002. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kämpfe J, Sedlmeier P, Renkewitz F. The impact of background music on adult listeners: A meta-analysis. Psychology of Music. 2011; 39 (4):424–448. doi: 10.1177/0305735610376261. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Küssner MB. Eysenck's theory of personality and the role of background music in cognitive task performance: A mini-review of conflicting findings and a new perspective. Frontiers in Psychology. 2017 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01991. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nantais KM, Schellenberg EG. The Mozart effect: An artifact of preference. Psychological Science. 1999; 10 (4):370–373. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00170. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • North AC, Hargreaves DJ. Music and driving game performance. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology. 1999; 40 (4):285–292. doi: 10.1111/1467-9450.404128. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Robertson HI, O'Connell R. Vigilant attention. In: Nobre CA, Coull TJ, editors. Attention and time. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2010. pp. 79–88. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Robertson IH, Manly T, Andrade J, Baddeley BT, Yiend J. "Oops!": Performance correlates of everyday attentional failures in traumatic brain injured and normal subjects. Neuropsychologia. 1997; 35 (6):747–758. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(97)00015-8. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rodrigues AC, Loureiro MA, Caramelli P. Long-term musical training may improve different forms of visual attention ability. Brain and Cognition. 2013; 82 (3):229–235. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2013.04.009. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Salame P, Baddeley A. Disruption of short-term memory by unattended speech—implicationg for the structure of working memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 1982; 21 (2):150–164. doi: 10.1016/s0022-5371(82)90521-7. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Shih YN, Huang RH, Chiang HY. Background music: Effects on attention performance. Work A Journal of Prevention Assessment & Rehabilitation. 2012; 42 (4):573–578. doi: 10.3233/wor-2012-1410. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Soares-Quadros Júnior JF, Lorenzo O, Herrera L, Araújo Santos NS. Gender and religion as factors of individual differences in musical preference. Musicae Scientiae. 2019; 23 (4):525–539. doi: 10.1177/1029864918774834. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Spotify AB. (2018). Spotify for Developers. https://developer.spotify.com . Retrieved 10 Jan 2019.
  • Turner ML, Fernandez JE, Nelson K. The effect of music amplitude on the reaction to unexpected visual events. The Journal of General Psychology. 1996; 123 (1):51–62. doi: 10.1080/00221309.1996.9921259. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ünal AB, de Waard D, Epstude K, Steg L. Driving with music: Effects on arousal and performance. Transportation Research Part F-Traffic Psychology and Behaviour. 2013; 21 :52–65. doi: 10.1016/j.trf.2013.09.004. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ünal AB, Steg L, Epstude K. The influence of music on mental effort and driving performance. Accident Analysis and Prevention. 2012; 48 :271–278. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2012.01.022. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Unsworth N, Robison MK. Pupillary correlates of lapses of sustained attention. Cognitive Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience. 2016; 16 (4):601–615. doi: 10.3758/s13415-016-0417-4. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Unsworth N, Robison MK. Tracking arousal state and mind wandering with pupillometry. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioural Neuroscience. 2018; 18 (4):638–664. doi: 10.3758/s13415-018-0594-4. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wang, D., Jimison, Z., Richard, D., & Chuan, C. H. (2015). Effect of listening to music as a function of driving complexity: A simulator study on the differing effects of music on different driving tasks. In Proceedings of the 8th international driving symposium on human factors in driver assessment, training, and vehicle design : driving assessment 2015 , Snowbird, Salt Lake City, Utah, USA, June 22–25, 2015.
  • Whyte J, Polansky M, Fleming M, Coslett HB, Cavallucci C. Sustained arousal and attention after traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychologia. 1995; 33 :797–813. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(96)00092-9. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Yerkes RM, Dodson JD. The relationship of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit formation. Journal of Comparative Neurology and Psychology. 1908; 18 :459–482. doi: 10.1002/cne.920180503. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]

Auralcrave

Stories for open-minded

essay about listening to music while studying

How music can affect your writing while studying

essay about listening to music while studying

A lot of students prefer to listen to music while studying, whether they are revising materials or writing an essay. There is a popular idea that melody can help one become smarter, which originates from 1993 research conducted by Dr. Gordon Shaw. The theory was called the Mozart Effect and claimed that listening to Mozart can increase the IQ of the students. However, the theory has never been proven and further findings show little connection and evidence of such effect, the idea remains vivid among people.

Music can affect our mood and performance, yet, it is important to understand how it actually works and in what circumstances. That will hep you when you are busy studying, in case you didn’t already choose to ask help to  essay writing services  in order to get the best result with less effort.

Listening to Music Improves the Mood

There are two main critique factors when we talk about the Mozart Effect. First of all, the tasks that the students were asked to perform in original research are connected to only one type of intelligence. Secondly, the results have never been achieved a second time. It means that, despite the fact, that theory is very attractive, it is not correct.

On the other hand, the melody does impact peoples’ moods. It evokes emotions and listening to a nice tune can make us feel better. Even if you are listening to a sad song, it can improve your mood. And, overall, a better mood is good for performing tasks like studying.

The other essential influence of music is that it reduces the level of cortisol in a body. Melody can and will decrease the level of stress one is experiencing. It is especially important for students that tend to deal with lots of stress in college or university. Listening to music before a study session can make you feel better and less stressed, which will result in more efficient work on the task.

Taking this data into consideration, one might use it to make writing more pleasant and effective. For example, you can listen for half an hour for a favorite artist and then proceed with working on an essay. It will not make one smarter instantly, but it will boost the overall well-being and make one more focused on the assignment.

Memorization and Sound

The link between memory and music is also yet to be explored more sufficiently. However, several studies show how memorization is influenced by background sounds.

There is evidence that students that tried to memorize information while the sound was on, found it harder to remember it in circumstances of an exam or test, where there was no music. Overall, it is better to memorize information in the environment that is similar to the one, where you will try to revoke it in your brain.

Students that learn materials in quiet performed better. There was also a difference between listening to tunes with lyrics, without them, the music that is loved by a student and the melodies that student doesn’t like. This leads us to the next point.

The Type of Music Matters

There is a huge difference between studying while listening to songs with or without lyrics. When you are reading or writing while the lyrics are present, the brain has to multitask. The brain works on two sources of information at the same time – processing lyrics and working on writing or reading. It decreases the concentration and makes writing less effective.

However, the classic music or any other type without any lyrics doesn’t have such an effect. Some findings prove that it is beneficial for creative writing . So, if you are working on a creative writing project, a melody might help to boost imagination and set the mood. It is influential in terms of evoking emotions. However, while working on a science project it might be not so helpful and more distracting. After all, you can always benefit from compare and contrast essay writing service if the task is too overbearing.

The crucial factor is also personal preference and habits. If you have always studied with the music on, it is probably something that works for you. Some people do find it easier to concentrate with background sound.

Music can help while writing in several ways – it reduces stress and anxiety, it makes the mood better, and it can boost imagination when working on a creative assignment. However, the type of tune impacts the efficiency of the study session; the songs with lyrics make us less effective while reading or writing. But listening to something without any words can be very helpful and soothing. After all, you do what works best for you because there is no universal recipe for every student.

Vasyl Kafidoff

Vasyl Kafidoff is a professional writer at WritingMetier. He is interested in education, modern techniques and the way technology shapes the academic process and makes students’ lives easier.

Share this story:

  • Click to share on Facebook (Opens in new window)
  • Click to share on Twitter (Opens in new window)
  • Click to share on Pocket (Opens in new window)

One comment

  • Pingback: I Can't Write Anymore! Stuck With Your Writing? | Kafidoff.com

Leave a Reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

This site uses Akismet to reduce spam. Learn how your comment data is processed .

About us | Privacy Policy | Contact us

essay about listening to music while studying

6 Advantages and Disadvantages of Listening to Music While Studying

essay about listening to music while studying

The music is one of the most inspiring and comprehensive arts. Since the beginning of history, man has had the drive to communicate his ideas and feelings through song and dance. And as for dancing, you will dance for joy if your assignment will write a service like Domyessay .

Nowadays, even employers from companies like BidForWriting , in order to do their work more efficiently, listen to music that helps them stay focused. Today there are hundreds of genres and millions of artists of all kinds. From popular and folk to classical, pop, rock, and jazz.

Regardless of your favorite, various research shows why you should listen while you study.

Here are the reasons why it benefits learning, but we also tell you how to listen in properly while studying and a few disadvantages.

1. It helps you memories better

essay about listening to music while studying

When we listen to music, our brain releases a chemical called dopamine, also known as the “happiness hormone.” And what does it have to do with learning? Memories have been shown to always contain an emotional element, so when you study listening to music, knowledge is more likely to be grounded in your brain.

2. Strengthen your cognitive ability

Following in line with the previous point, recent research determined that people who learn to play a musical instrument have better results in variables such as vocabulary, critical reasoning ability, and fine motor skills. So you should also learn to play it and not just listen to it!

3. Stimulates concentration

essay about listening to music while studying

It is known that many surgeons, when operating, listen to their favorite songs as a way to block any distraction. Various studies have shown that listening to calm and harmonious music while doing intellectual tasks that require a lot of concentration is a highly effective method.

What genre should you listen to?

Relaxing music for studying is the best. Styles like classical or background, chill out or movie soundtracks come into play here.

Better instrumental than with lyrics

essay about listening to music while studying

Let’s not fool ourselves. If we put on a familiar song, the kind that continually plays on bars and on TV, we won’t be able to keep our attention on the sheets in front of us and humming will engulf the room sooner than later. Avoid such distractions and make sure your playlist is made up of instrumental themes, it’s the best music to study and focus on.

Listen to it to perform mechanical tasks

Relaxing music to study is a good companion to the most mechanical, repetitive, and monotonous memory tasks. But we must be aware that meaningful learning, the truly resulting from the understanding of the contents and subsequent appropriation of knowledge, is best done in a silent environment.

Play it if there is too much noise around

essay about listening to music while studying

Linking the previous idea, before the impossibility of silence, relaxing music is better to study than noise. Our house or library is not always quiet places, so put on music and get a stable and unalterable environment.

Control the volume, you’re not partying – It should be at a medium-low volume. Forget about full volume.

Use long playlists – If the playlist is constantly stopped and you have to interrupt what you are doing to change to another one, don’t worry. Think about the time you usually study without break and make a list of this duration so that it ends when you go to take a break. The average is in 2 or 3 hours. Or just play some radio station, like ChilloutRadio , and let them choose for you appropriate music.

Disadvantages

4. multitasking.

essay about listening to music while studying

Multitasking is a term that has its origin in the field of computing. By multitasking, we understand the process by which two or more tasks or processes are performed at the same time.

The term multitasking is applied in different aspects related to productivity and performance and, unfortunately, it is becoming a quality highly valued in the academic and business world.

But people with multitasking behavior lose concentration as the brain clearly conflicts. In this sense and in relation to the topic of this article, listening to music while studying is detrimental because the brain must be divided into two actions.

On the one hand, the brain works on aspects of the study, while on the other, it listens to songs. The effect is just the opposite of what people think. Listening while studying does not help us relax or concentrate, on the contrary, it increases our stress and decreases our attention.

5. Productivity

In relation to the first reason is that by doing two tasks simultaneously you lose time and productivity.

Many people find it that if it is silence around them, they are achieving much better results. But that is probably on the personal level, everyone has different preferences. The music is still noise and all noise that it causes is an alteration in the brain. For a long time, when doing an intellectual activity, it makes it extremely difficult for me to do any type of task related to work.

essay about listening to music while studying

Today many of the people who listen to music do so through electronic devices such as players, mobile devices, tablets…

They store a huge amount of data on these devices. This music, which will be the one chosen to listen to while studying, becomes continuous decision-making. I explain. By having so much musical information on our device, we waste a remarkable amount of time selecting those songs that we like. What this selection process does is to continuously interrupt the pace of study and directly affects the concentration required by a task such as studying.

The problem of these devices is also linked to our tastes, because while we listen to music, the songs evoke some kind of feeling, memory, taste, aversion, etc.

All these blocks and interrupts the study again and makes the time we dedicate to study increase considerably. When talking about the expression of listening to music, you have to make a clear differentiation.

Conclusion:

Music has fundamental benefits for the brain . Recent research indicates that learning to play an instrument in childhood manages to prosper a person’s cognitive performance in each and every aspect. In addition to this, it has also been proven that it compensates for the cognitive loss of aging.

This is all because music produces neural connections that improve other aspects of communication.

Related Posts:

  • 9 Advantages And Disadvantages Of Using Stainless…
  • The Advantages and Disadvantages of Playing at a…
  • Unlock the Potential of Social Listening for Maximum Impact
  • Studying for the Sake Professional Certification ─…
  • How Early Should You Start Studying for the GRE?
  • Mathematics in Art and Music ─ The Harmony of…

RELATED ARTICLES MORE FROM AUTHOR

essay about listening to music while studying

Revolutionizing the Classroom ─ How STEM Education Shapes Future Innovators

essay about listening to music while studying

5 Advices For Online Students

essay about listening to music while studying

What Is a Claim in an Essay? Unpacking the Core Element with Writing Tips

essay about listening to music while studying

How to Maximize Your Digital Marketing ─ 5 Tips for Success...

essay about listening to music while studying

Is Body Massaging a Profitable Career Choice? Analyzing Income Potential

essay about listening to music while studying

A Homeowner’s Guide to Buying a Riding Lawn Mower

Hyperbaric Oxygen Therapy

What Is the Success Rate of Hyperbaric Oxygen Therapy? 3 Tips...

essay about listening to music while studying

A Guide to Understanding Workplace Policies and Compliance

essay about listening to music while studying

Listening to Music While Studying: Can Eric Stewart Music Help You Write an Essay?

essay about listening to music while studying

Probably each of us has certain rituals or tricks that help us concentrate and become our most productive selves while studying. For some, it might be a short walk or exercising. But, one of the most commonly used tricks is listening to the right kind of background music while studying and writing essays, in particular.

Does listening to music help you study? It might. Numerous studies confirm that listening to certain types of music can have a very positive effect on one’s brain, boosting cognitive functions and helping you stay focused longer.

According to our survey, 87% of school and college students prefer listening to music while writing their papers. The majority of them prefer the music by Eric Stewart, and all of them are confident that it helps them study better.

So, can listening to music really help me write my essay for me ? And, if so, what kind of essay music is the best? If these are the questions you have in mind, you’ve come to the right place because this article will tell you about all the benefits of listening to music while writing essays and the benefits of listening to Eric Stewart 10cc, in particular!

Eric Stewart Music for Writing Essays

So, why should you listen to music while studying? It is proven that listening to music while studying can have real benefits. But, the big question is what kind of music to choose?

As we mentioned earlier, a survey among students has shown that a large share of them finds music to be very helpful in studying and writing essays. Many respondents also highlighted that an important place in their playlists for studying is taken by a famous musician Eric Stewart. What makes his songs a great option for your own “essay writing music” playlist?

As over 60% of students from the institutions noted, the music by Eric Stewart 10cc makes it “easier to study.” There are several reasons for that. Since 10cc is an iconic British music band from the 70s, many students know their music pretty well. And, if you are wondering how this helps, specialists recommend listening to essay music you know in order not to get distracted by it.

Secondly, Eric Stewart and his band have a very broad repertoire that contains songs to match all tastes. This makes it easy for everyone to find something that works for them.

Finally, one more reason why respondents said they enjoy listening to Eric Stewart while studying is that his energetic and fun tunes can help you get in the right mood for essay writing and stay motivated.

Sounds good enough to try it yourself, right? But wait, there is more! Read on to learn about the real benefits of listening to music while writing essays, about the possible cons, and about a reliable essay writing service that can help even when music isn’t helping.

essay about listening to music while studying

Pros of Using Music for Writing Essays

Unless you are writing an essay on music, it may not be very obvious to play some songs in the background while working on it. However, you might be surprised by how many benefits it can give you if you have the right playlist for custom writing and studying.

music

Namely, there are 3 major perks you can enjoy if you play background music while writing your academic papers:

  • Better focus - Although some may find music distracting, it can be very helpful in terms of getting focused because it can help you control all the other distractions and reduce the noise.
  • Improved mood - Probably no one likes writing papers because it can be very boring, stressful, and not really fun whatsoever. However, with the right playlist that includes your favorite artists like Eric Stewart and others, you can boost your mood.
  • Less stress - Finally, scientists have long proven that music can help you defeat stress and reduce anxiety.

All these benefits prove that listening to music can be helpful not only when you are assigned with essay writing on music, in particular, but also in many other cases when you need to write a paper quickly, easily, and with no stress.

Cons of Listening to Essay Writing Music

Despite a huge number of possible benefits, we have to admit that listening to music while studying might not be a thing for everyone. Even if you choose good essay writing music, there is always a chance that it can do more harm than good.

music

Here are some of the main shortcomings of this approach that you may face:

  • Unwanted distraction - Some people just can’t stand extraneous sounds during work or studies. In this case, what was supposed to help you turns into a huge distraction and can’t do any good.
  • Decreased productivity - Since it can be a major distraction, listening to music while studying can have a negative effect on your productivity.
  • Doesn’t work for all kinds of tasks - Finally, one more drawback is the fact that listening to music may not be helpful on all occasions as there are some tasks that require maximum concentration. For example, reading a book can be rather hard if you listen to music with words.

These are the main cons you may face. But, is there a way to avoid them, you may wonder? Probably the only way to avoid issues is to experiment and find what works best for you.

Can Music Motivate You to Write?

So, is it good to listen to music while studying? Music is proven to have a whole range of effects on the human body, both on physical and emotional levels.

When you listen to the music you actually like, it boosts your mood and can even make you more motivated. Thus, if you really enjoy Eric Stuart’s music, go for it, and it will help you stay motivated and, thus, do better with your assignments. So, the main thing students should keep in mind when deciding whether it is worth playing some background music while writing an essay is to give preference to something they like.

Also, playing your favorite songs in the background can make the process less boring. This way, you will stop perceiving essay writing as a form of torture and may finally start enjoying it, which also influences motivation.

Besides, studies show that listening to music can significantly reduce stress levels, which, as we all know, always rise when one is working on such a complex and time-consuming task as essay writing.

Thus, based on everything that has been said, the answer to the question, “can music help you study?” is definitely yes, it can!

Have Troubles With Writing Your Essay?

Although listening to music can help you write an essay, if this doesn’t work, we have a solution that will definitely help in any situation.

When you are running out of time or can’t handle your task, our essay service is all you need! Turning to our EssayService, students can pay for essay and get professionally written paper from top-level writers to boost their grades. Entrust us to write your essay on music or any other subject and we will help you succeed!

Frequently asked questions

She was flawless! first time using a website like this, I've ordered article review and i totally adored it! grammar punctuation, content - everything was on point

This writer is my go to, because whenever I need someone who I can trust my task to - I hire Joy. She wrote almost every paper for me for the last 2 years

Term paper done up to a highest standard, no revisions, perfect communication. 10s across the board!!!!!!!

I send him instructions and that's it. my paper was done 10 hours later, no stupid questions, he nailed it.

Sometimes I wonder if Michael is secretly a professor because he literally knows everything. HE DID SO WELL THAT MY PROF SHOWED MY PAPER AS AN EXAMPLE. unbelievable, many thanks

You Might Also Like

Sports Psychology Graduate Programs

New Posts to Your Inbox!

Stay in touch

Effects of listening to music in exam-taking environments for engineering students

  • Published: 08 October 2022
  • Volume 28 , pages 3977–4001, ( 2023 )

Cite this article

  • David Claudio   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0001-8068-5932 1 &
  • Morten Bakke 2  

1593 Accesses

Explore all metrics

This research explores the effects of listening to music during exams for engineering college students. Students were given the option to listen to self-selected music while completing exams in three undergraduate engineering courses over four years. It was found that listening to music during an exam had no significant effect on the mean exam scores or completion times. However, when analyzing data from students who had taken exams both with and without music, it was found that these students performed significantly better in the music condition but at par with the mean score. Results indicate that allowing music during exams for students who study with music does not represent an advantage but rather an opportunity for equity in exam-taking environments.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price includes VAT (Russian Federation)

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Rent this article via DeepDyve

Institutional subscriptions

essay about listening to music while studying

Similar content being viewed by others

essay about listening to music while studying

The Effects of Background Music Style on Study Performance

essay about listening to music while studying

Music for all for Music

essay about listening to music while studying

Modern music production equipment and how it can be used in student teaching: overture and its impact on motivation and interest in electronic music creation

Baole Huang

Data availability

All data, materials, and software applications or custom code support the published claims and comply with field standards.

Ahlawat, S., Batra, P., & Sharma, A. (2012). Personality and music: An effect on arithmetic performance. Indian Journal of Positive Psychology, 3 (1), 96–100.

Google Scholar  

Angel, L. A., Polzella, D. J., & Elvers, G. C. (2010). Background music and cognitive performance. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 110 (3C), 1059–1064.

Article   Google Scholar  

Bridgett, D.J. and Cuevas, J. (2000). Effects of listening to mozart and bach on the performance of a mathematical test. Perceptual and Motor Skills , 90(3_suppl), 1171–1175.

Cassidy, G. G., & Macdonald, R. A. (2010). The effects of music on time perception and performance of a driving game. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 51 (6), 455–464.

Chou, P. (2010). Attention drainage effect: How background music effects concentration in Taiwanese college students. Journal of the Scholarship of Teaching and Learning, 10 (1), 36–46.

Cockerham, D., Lin, L., Zhengsi, C., & Schellen, M. (2019). Cross-sectional studies investigating the impacts of background sounds on cognitive task performance. Mind, Brain and Technology (pp. 177–194). Cham, Switzerland: Springer.

Chapter   Google Scholar  

Crawford, H. J., & Strapp, C. M. (1994). Effects of vocal and instrumental music on visuospatial and verbal performance as moderated by studying preference and personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 16 , 237–245.

Culbert, S. S., & Posner, M. I. (1960). Human habituation to an acoustical energy distribution spectrum. Journal of Applied Psychology, 44 , 263–266.

Daoussis, L., & McKelvie, S. J. (1986). Musical preferences and effects of music on a reading comprehension test for extraverts and introverts. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 62 (1), 283–289.

Datta, H., Knox, G., & Bronnenberg, B. J. (2018). Changing their Tune: how consumers’ adoption of online streaming affects music consumption and discovery. Marketing Science, 37 (1), 5–21.

Demorest, S. M., & Morrison, S. J. (2016). Does music make you smarter? Music Educators Journal, 87 (2), 33–58.

Dobbs, S., Furnham, A., & McClelland, A. (2011). The effect of background music and noise on the cognitive test performance of introverts and extraverts. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25 (2), 307–313.

Dolegui, A. S. (2013). The impact of listening to music on cognitive performance. Inquiries Journal, 5 (9), 1.

Etaugh, C., & Michals, D. (1975). Effects on reading comprehension of preferred music and frequency of studying to music. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 41 , 553–554.

Feller, W. (1948). On the kolmogorov-smirnov limit theorems for empirical distributions. Annals of Mathematical Statistics, 19 , 177–189.

Article   MathSciNet   MATH   Google Scholar  

Furnham, A., & Strbac, L. (2002). Music is as distracting as noise: The differential distraction of background music and noise on the cognitive test performance of introverts and extraverts. Ergonomics, 45 (3), 203–217.

Gonzalez, M. F., & Aiello, J. R. (2019). More than meets the ear: Investigating how music affects cognitive task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 25 (3), 431–444.

Gurung, R. (2002). How do students really study (and does it matter)? Journal of Personality Teaching of Psychology Communication Quarterly Communication Education, 2 , 149–155.

Hall, J. C. (1952). The effect of background music on the reading comprehension of 278 eighth and ninth grade students. The Journal of Educational Research, 45 (6), 451–458.

Hallam, S., & Price, J. (1998). Research section: Can the use of background music improve the behaviour and academic performance of children with emotional and behavioural difficulties? British Journal of Special Education, 25 (2), 88–91.

Hallam, S., Price, J., & Katsarou, G. (2002). The Effects of background music on primary school pupils’ task performance. Educational Studies, 28 (2), 111–122.

Hilliard, O. M., & Tolin, P. (1979). Effect of familiarity with background music on performance of simple and difficult reading comprehension tasks. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 49 , 713–714.

Jäncke, L., & Sandmann, P. (2010). Music listening while you learn: No influence of background music on verbal learning. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 6 (1), 3.

Kotsopoulou, A., & Hallam, S. (2010). The perceived impact of playing music while studying: Age and cultural differences. Educational Studies, 36 (4), 431–440.

Kumar, N., Wajidi, M. A., Chian, Y. T., Vishroothi, S., Swamy, R. S., & Aithal, A. P. (2016). The effect of listening to music on concentration and academic performance of the student: Cross-sectional study on medical undergraduate students. Research Journal of Pharmaceutical, Biological and Chemical Sciences, 7 , 1190–1195.

Martin, R. C., Wogalter, M. S., & Forlano, J. G. (1988). Reading comprehension in the presence of unattended speech and music. Journal of Memory and Language, 27 (4), 382–398.

Mayfield, C., and Moss, S. (1989). Effect of music tempo on task performance. Psychological Reports , 65(3 Pt 2):1283–1290.  https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1989.65.3f.1283

Pinto, M., Sales, D., Fernández-Pascual, R., & Caballero-Mariscal, D. (2020a). Attitudes, perceptions and prospectings on mobile information literacy training: Design and validation of the MOBILE-APP questionnaire. Journal of Librarianship and Information Science, 52 (1), 208–223.

Pinto, M., Caballero, D., Sales, D., & Fernández-Pascual, R. (2020b). MOBILE-APPS questionnaire: Developing and validating a scale to measure the attitudes and perceptions of undergraduate students on mobile information literacy. Journal of Librarianship and Information Science, 52 (4), 1063–1072.

Rauscher, F. H., & Shaw, G. L. (1998). Key components of the mozart effect. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 86 (3), 835–841.

Rauscher, F. H., Shaw, G. L., & Ky, C. N. (1993). Music and spatial task performance. Nature, 365 (6447), 611–611.

Rauscher, F. H., Shaw, G. L., & Ky, K. N. (1995). Listening to Mozart enhances spatial-temporal reasoning: Towards a neurophysiological basis. Neuroscience Letters, 185 (1), 44–47.

Rideout, B. E., & Laubach, C. M. (1996). EEG correlates of enhanced spatial performance following exposure to music. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 82 (2), 427–432.

Salamé, P. and. Baddeley, A.D, (1989). Effects of background music on phonological short-term memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology , 41(1-A),107–122.

Schellenberg, E. G., Nakata, T., Hunter, P. G., & Tamoto, S. (2007). Exposure to music and cognitive performance: Tests of children and adults. Psychology of Music, 35 (1), 5–19.

Statistics Solutions. (2013). Normality . Retrieved from  https://www.statisticssolutions.com/academic-solutions/resources/directory-of-statistical-analyses/normality/ . Accessed 9 Feb 2020.

Thompson, W. F., Schellenberg, E. G., & Letnic, A. K. (2012). Fast and loud background music disrupts reading comprehension. Psychology of Music, 40 (6), 700–708.

Vaughn, K. (2000). Music, and mathematics: Modest support for the oft-claimed relationship. Journal of Aesthetic Education, 34 (3/4), 149.

Widerman, M.S. (2013). Study habits and music: How they affect attention and academic performance.  http://ebot.gmu.edu/handle/1920/8322 . Accessed 8 Feb 2020.

Yau, N. (2011). Visualize this: The flowing data guide to design, visualization, and statistics, 1st ed. Indianapolis, Ind. : Wiley: Indianapolis, Ind.

Download references

The authors did not receive support from any organization for the submitted work.

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Department of Mechanical and Industrial Engineering, University of Massachusetts Lowell, Lowell, MA, 01854, USA

David Claudio

Department of Mechanical and Industrial Engineering, Montana State University, Bozeman, MT, 59717, USA

Morten Bakke

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Contributions

All authors contributed to the study's conception and design. Material preparation, and data collection were performed by David Claudio. Analysis was performed by Morten Bakke and David Claudio. The first draft of the manuscript was written by Morten Bakke, and all authors commented on previous versions of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to David Claudio .

Ethics declarations

Conflicts of interest/competing interests.

One of the researchers was also the instructor of all three courses mentioned in the article. The authors declare they have no financial interests. Ethical approval was waived by the local Ethics Committee of University A in view of the retrospective nature of the study and all the procedures being performed were part of the routine care.

Additional information

Publisher's note.

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Claudio, D., Bakke, M. Effects of listening to music in exam-taking environments for engineering students. Educ Inf Technol 28 , 3977–4001 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10639-022-11375-z

Download citation

Received : 31 January 2022

Accepted : 26 September 2022

Published : 08 October 2022

Issue Date : April 2023

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/s10639-022-11375-z

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Background noise
  • Study habits
  • Concentration
  • Find a journal
  • Publish with us
  • Track your research

Amy Isaman

i would like to try

' src=

Hi Shawna – what do you want to try? Writing to different types of music?

' src=

I like to listen to music without lyrics while writing and do it about 50 to 60% of the time. It gets me in the mood and makes writing more fun for me.

I try to choose something, that fits, what I’m writing like dark music for a Thriller or Medieval Music for Fantasy. Ambient Noise for non-fiction stuff.

The thing is, it is much better to write with music than background noise. I like to keep the window open when I write and during the summer it can be a bit noisy. So I’d rather have music than having the window closed the whole day.

People need to figure it out by themselves in my opinion, it works differently for everyone.

It’s funny that none of the research studies I found talked about listening to music that “matched” what you’re writing and if that helps. I can think of several writer friends, and now you too, that do this. Do you have playlists you use for different genres? That might be kind of fun to put together!

And I agree, people need to figure it out by themselves, but with that said, the research was pretty clear on the fact that music without lyrics was the best. Thanks for reading!!!

[…] Should You Listen to Music While Writing? […]

Leave a Comment Cancel Reply

Save my name, email, and website in this browser for the next time I comment.

Privacy Overview

IMAGES

  1. Is it Good to Listen to Music While Studying? Free Essay Example

    essay about listening to music while studying

  2. 5 Benefits of Listening to Music when Studying Infographic

    essay about listening to music while studying

  3. 5 Benefits of Listening to Music when Studying Infographic

    essay about listening to music while studying

  4. The Music Guide: What You Should Listen To While Studying

    essay about listening to music while studying

  5. Music and Studying (600 Words)

    essay about listening to music while studying

  6. 5 Benefits of Listening to Music While Studying

    essay about listening to music while studying

VIDEO

  1. Should You Listen To Music While Studying? 🤔

  2. listening music while nag22pi

  3. Music to vibe to while studying

  4. Listening Music for writing Eternal Ephemera Book 3

  5. Listening music while studying Good or Bad ? Student Exam Study Tips by Vineet Khatri Sir

  6. LISTENING MUSIC WHILE AT WORKING||HAVE A GREAT DAY

COMMENTS

  1. Studying With Music: Arguments for & Against

    For example, try listening to music when rewriting or editing a paper. You'll quickly see that you work more quickly and efficiently when listening to music. Music can also make a boring activity seem less boring. Listening to music while doing something mundane, like creating charting tables or creating columns for Cornell notes.

  2. Does Listening to Music Really Help You Study?

    Even though experts suggest listening to music can hinder your ability to retain information while studying, some students choose to continue the practice. Steven Smith, cognitive neuroscientist for the Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences, provided some suggestions for students who wish to continue this study habit.

  3. Essay About Music While Studying

    Essay About Music While Studying. 913 Words4 Pages. Music today is being used all of the time, and now we can have it. Wherever we are, we have music, but the main question asked is if that's a good thing or not. Many people enjoy using music as background noise when studying, some of these people may not realize it, but the music could be ...

  4. Do you listen to music while studying? A portrait of how people use

    1. Introduction. Music is omnipresent in the current society. In 2018, an average Dutch person spent 152 min every day listening to music (The Statistical Portal, 2019) and the number of active users of music streaming services has significantly increased.For example, from 2015 to 2020, the number of Spotify users increased almost fivefold, from 68 to 320 million (The Statistical Portal, 2020).

  5. Impact of Music on Student's Academic Performance.

    Antony, Vishnu and gayatri (2018) concludes that music plays a major role in the academic performance of c ollege students. 40.5% of. the students listen to music while studying. 64% of students ...

  6. The Effects of Listening to Music While Studying Research Paper

    Research suggests that listening to music while studying may "distract attention from the studied material, thereby impeding learning." (Tan, L. 1999). Music is every where in the life of an individual, starting from the fetus stage till the end of the life. People listen to different types of music depending on their mood, and the effects ...

  7. Can music improve focus and concentration when studying?

    Listening to music when studying or concentrating may not affect each person in the same way. The genre, volume, and tempo of the music may all play a role in whether music helps people study.

  8. The Influence of Background Music on Learning in the Light of Different

    In this study we define background music as music that plays in the background while studying, i.e., when reading a text. Learners are intended to listen to this music but there is no relation between the music itself and the main task, namely learning the text. ... Music listening while you learn: no influence of background music on verbal ...

  9. 2019

    To sum up: research suggests it's probably fine to listen to music while you're studying - with some caveats. It's better if: it puts you in a good mood. it's not too fast or too loud. it's less wordy (and hip-hop, where the words are rapped rather than sung, is likely to be even more distracting) you're not too introverted.

  10. PDF Music and Memory: Effects of Listening to Music While Studying in

    have found detriments to studying while listening to music. One such study investigated the effects of multi-tasking and performance. Variables that were looked at included; listening to music while writing a work report or any other tasks, or other stimuli physical or visual, around the main task that interrupt the main task.

  11. The effect of preferred background music on task-focus in sustained

    Listening to background music while performing tasks that require sustained attention is common, although previous research on the benefit of such music listening is inconsistent. The current study aimed to explore how the effects of preferred background music are mediated via arousal and how music affects attentional state.

  12. Is it Good to Listen to Music While Studying?

    Music that is soothing and relaxing can help students to beat stress or anxiety while studying. Background music may improve focus on a task by providing motivation and improving mood. During long ...

  13. The Benefits and Deficits of Listening to Music While Studying

    listening to music while memorizing. If the song they listen to has high arousal, they are more. prone to focusing on the song than memorizing or working on schoolwork (Serpil Umuzdas, 2015) and thus, will not retain the information as well, making the technique less effective. As a.

  14. Background Music and Cognitive Task Performance: A Systematic Review of

    Amongst these activities, some of the most common ones involve mental work that require intensive cognitive functioning. For instance, Calderwood et al. (2014) conducted a study to understand what other activities students normally engage with whilst studying and found that, in a 3-hr study session, students spent more than one-third of the time (73 min) listening to music.

  15. The effect of listening to music on concentration and academic

    However, a researcher stated that listening to music while studying helps students concentrate on their studies and might improve their academic performance (Kumar et al., 2016). More research has ...

  16. How music can affect your writing while studying

    In Summary. Music can help while writing in several ways - it reduces stress and anxiety, it makes the mood better, and it can boost imagination when working on a creative assignment. However, the type of tune impacts the efficiency of the study session; the songs with lyrics make us less effective while reading or writing.

  17. Should You Listen to Music While Studying?

    Basically, listening to music increases your cognitive load. So when studying subjects that require a lot of reading and writing (like English and History), music makes it harder to process these words. If you find yourself having to read a paragraph over and over just to comprehend it, while listening to music, you should probably press pause ...

  18. (PDF) EFFECTS OF BACKGROUND MUSIC IN COMPLETING ACADEMIC ...

    Majority of adolescent read, study for test, and complete their homework while listening to music. Listening to music while studying is known as multitasking. Focus and ability to multitask play a ...

  19. 6 Advantages and Disadvantages of Listening to Music While Studying

    On the one hand, the brain works on aspects of the study, while on the other, it listens to songs. The effect is just the opposite of what people think. Listening while studying does not help us ...

  20. Essay Writing Music: Listen to Eric Stewart While Writing an Essay

    Numerous studies confirm that listening to certain types of music can have a very positive effect on one's brain, boosting cognitive functions and helping you stay focused longer. According to our survey, 87% of school and college students prefer listening to music while writing their papers. The majority of them prefer the music by Eric ...

  21. Effects of listening to music in exam-taking environments for

    This research explores the effects of listening to music during exams for engineering college students. Students were given the option to listen to self-selected music while completing exams in three undergraduate engineering courses over four years. It was found that listening to music during an exam had no significant effect on the mean exam scores or completion times. However, when ...

  22. Should You Listen to Music While Writing?

    For example, if you're cleaning the house, listening to music will help you get it done. Part of the reason for this is that great music puts us in a better mood which helps us stay focused and productive. But writing, isn't repetitive. It's a creative task. So only listen to music when you write i f you have a solid background in music.